National Academies Press: OpenBook

Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility (2004)

Chapter: 4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl

« Previous: 3 Health Consequences of Adolescent Alcohol Involvement--Sandra A. Brown and Susan F. Tapert
Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×

4
Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview

Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl

How do we prevent underage drinking in a society in which youth receive positive messages about alcohol coupled with warnings that alcohol is illegal and risky? Alcohol pervades our culture, and youth are well aware of its popularity. Adolescents and young adults witness the casual use of alcohol by parents, other adults, and peers, and alcohol is commonly displayed in advertisements, in movies, on television, on t-shirts, at sports events, and through similar venues. Therefore, it is not surprising that experimental or occasional use of alcohol is reported by the majority of U.S. adolescents (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2001), making it, by definition, a normative behavior during the second decade of life. This is not to say that underage alcohol consumption is condoned, as any alcohol use can lead to cognitive impairment, risk of injury or death if consumed before vehicle use, or violence. Futhermore, it is illegal for anyone under age 21.

In this chapter, we provide an overview of the developmental and environmental factors that have been implicated in normative underage alcohol use.1 In doing so, we apply a lifespan developmental systems approach (Baltes, 1987; Baltes, Reese, and Lipsitt, 1980; Lerner et al., 1994), whereby development is viewed as a lifespan process, with past events

1  

We do not discuss factors that influence excessive or atypical alcohol use (e.g., mental or addictive disorders) because the factors generally responsible for these drinking patterns are not necessarily the same as those involved in normative alcohol use.

Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×

influencing current ones, which in turn influence future developments. A central tenet of lifespan models is that there is a dynamic, reciprocal interaction between the individual and his or her environment. Therefore, reducing risk behavior and promoting healthy pathways can only be accomplished through a fundamental understanding of the developmental and environmental factors that impinge on youths’ lives.

In this chapter, we do not attempt to review all of the literature concerning the multitude of factors involved in adolescent alcohol use. Instead, we focus on developmental and environmental influences, with particular emphasis on factors we consider most amenable to prevention and intervention efforts. We refer readers to other, more extensive and lengthy reviews that have already made significant contributions to the field (e.g., Dielman, Butchart, Shope, and Miller, 1991; Halebsky, 1987; Hawkins, Catalano, and Miller, 1992; Vakalahi, 2001).

DEVELOPMENTAL INFLUENCES ON UNDERAGE ALCOHOL USE

Adolescence marks a period of great and rapid physical, cognitive, psychosocial, and emotional changes. All of these changes increase one’s desire for more autonomy and decision making, which can result in risk taking, including alcohol use. Understanding how these individual-level developmental factors relate to alcohol use is critical in order to create developmentally appropriate and effective alcohol prevention and intervention programs. In this section, we review adolescent physical, cognitive, and psychosocial development, with particular emphasis on how such development influences underage alcohol consumption.

Physical Development

Adolescent development is typically marked by rapid and extreme biological changes—namely, the onset of puberty and menarche. During this time, hormones, brain development, and the environment interact to result in increased growth, change in voice characteristics, and the development of secondary sex characteristics.

Puberty often signals changes in one’s own as well as others’ expectations for the adolescent. For example, taller and more mature-looking adolescents are often expected to be more responsible and take on adult roles and behaviors earlier than less physically mature peers of the same age group. However, depending on the age at which puberty occurs, the adolescents’ level of social and emotional maturation may not match their physical development. When this mismatch occurs, expectations for adult-like responsibilities may be met with negative or health-compromising self-perceptions and behaviors. For example, early maturing girls tend to show

Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×

increased dissatisfaction with their bodies, have lower self-esteem, have school difficulties, and engage in problem behaviors such as drinking, smoking, and early sexual behavior as compared to late-maturing girls. In contrast, late-maturing boys express more dissatisfaction with their body image, tend to be less popular and less athletic, and perform less well academically than early maturing boys. The psychological and behavioral difficulties resulting from early or late pubertal onset can be ameliorated through parental involvement, communication, and support.

Cognitive and Psychosocial Development

During adolescence, thinking becomes more abstract and less concrete, and adolescents become more future oriented, allowing them to consider multiple aspects of any decision at one time, assess potential consequences of a decision, consider possible outcomes associated with various choices, and plan for the future. These cognitive changes are coupled with psychosocial development, including social perspective taking, susceptibility to peer pressure, and increased need for autonomy (e.g., Steinberg and Cauffman, 1996). Social perspective taking refers to the ability to recognize how the thoughts and actions of one person can influence those of another. Social perspective taking has been shown to gradually increase until about age 16 (Steinberg and Cauffman, 1996). Applied to underage drinking, it is expected that individuals who are more capable of social perspective taking will be more able to understand why underage alcohol use is not condoned and understand that not all people have the same views concerning alcohol use. Although generally an indicator of greater maturity, a downside of this new ability to understand different perspectives is that adolescents become highly concerned with peer conformity, which may make them more susceptible to peer pressure, including pressure to drink alcohol. A more detailed discussion of peer influences and social norms is presented later in this chapter.

These cognitive and psychosocial changes are also accompanied by adolescents’ questioning of parental control and rules and their desire to be more autonomous, which often translates into their desire to participate in, and eventually dictate, their own decision making. The desire to be autonomous and make one’s own decisions is considered an important hallmark of adolescence. The literature clearly indicates that the need to be more autonomous and be granted more decision making opportunities increases with age. Several other simultaneous changes during adolescence serve to increase adolescents’ desire for autonomy. First, as indicated earlier, physical changes of puberty result in the adolescent seeing himself or herself as more deserving of privileges, and others tend to have similar expectations for youth. Second, increased time spent with peers leads to more experi-

Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×

ences and comparisons of others’ authority, power, and privileges. Finally, cultural and societal beliefs indicate that adolescence is a time to practice adult roles (Hill, 1973; see also Silverberg and Gondoli, 1996, for a review).

Perceptions of Alcohol-Related Risks

Two critical and overlapping areas of theory and research have emerged from our understanding of adolescent cognitive and psychosocial development: 1) adolescents’ perceptions of risk, and 2) adolescent decision making. These areas have particular relevance to clinicians, policy makers, and researchers interested in understanding underage alcohol use. In this chapter, we discuss the notion of adolescent egocentrism, perceptions of invulnerability, and alcohol-related expectancies. For a discussion regarding alcohol-related decision making, see Jacobs, Chapter 5.

Both lay and scientific circles often have hypothesized that adolescents are not concerned about their health, that they make poor judgments about risks, and that they believe they are invulnerable to harm, and because of these perceptions, they engage in risk behaviors (see, for example, Social Cognitive Theory [Bandura, 1994], the Health Belief Model [Rosenstock, 1974], the Theory of Reasoned Action [Fishbein and Ajzen, 1975], the Theory of Planned Behavior [Ajzen, 1985], Self-Regulation Theory [Kanfer, 1970], and Subjective Culture and Interpersonal Relations Theory [Triandis, 1977]). The notion that adolescents do not adequately perceive risks largely stems from Elkind (1967, 1978), who argued that egocentric young adolescents hold an exaggerated sense of uniqueness and believe in a “personal fable”—that they are special and in some ways immune to the natural laws that pertain to others. Thus, they view themselves as invulnerable to harm, and therefore are more likely to engage in risk behaviors. Adolescent egocentrism and the associated perceptions of invulnerability are thought to become less evident as the adolescent matures cognitively.

Although Elkind’s theory is appealing, it has not held up to empirical scrutiny. Evidence shows that adolescent egocentrism increases from childhood to early adolescence, and then gradually decreases through middle and late adolescence (De Rosenroll, 1987; Urberg and Robbins, 1984). But adolescent egocentrism does not appear to be linked to young adolescents’ transition into formal operations (Gray and Hudson, 1984; Jahnke and Blanchard-Fields, 1993; Lapsley, Milstead, Quintana, Flannery, and Buss, 1986), nor has research supported a relationship between egocentrism and perceptions of invulnerability (Dolcini et al., 1989).

The majority of studies assessing the relationship between perceptions of invulnerability to harm and alcohol use have examined whether judgments of risk vary by behavioral engagement. Overall, results show that adolescents who have drunk alcohol perceive alcohol-related risks to be less

Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×

likely to occur compared to adolescents without prior alcohol consumption (Goldberg, Halpern-Felsher, and Millstein, 2002; Halpern-Felsher et al., 2001; Hampson, Severson, Burns, Slovic, and Fisher, 2001; Kuther, 2002). Another set of studies has examined whether adolescents and adults differ in their estimates of risk. Contrary to hypotheses, most of this research has revealed that adults and adolescents give similar estimates of risk taking, including those related to alcohol use (e.g., Beyth-Marom, Austin, Fischhoff, Palmgren, and Jacobs-Quadrel, 1993; Quadrel, Fischhoff, and Davis, 1993).

Causal relationships between risk estimates and alcohol use must be made cautiously as most studies have used cross-sectional methodology, poor definitions of alcohol use, and inadequate assessments of risk perceptions (see Halpern-Felsher et al., 2001, for details). Despite lack of evidence, the concept of adolescent “invulnerability” remains pervasive in both scientific and lay circles, is used to explain adolescents’ decisions to engage in potentially harmful behavior, and is incorporated into many intervention programs (Weinstein, 1983; Weisenberg, Kegeles, and Lund, 1980). Longitudinal, prospective studies are needed in order to fully understand the extent to which perceptions of low risk actually predict and motivate alcohol use.

Perceptions of Alcohol-Related Benefits

An emphasis on perceived risk alone may be inadequate to predict or change behavior because risk is only part of the behavioral decision making equation. What is missing is knowing the extent to which adolescents perceive benefits of risky behaviors. The decision making literature has argued that individuals should consider both the risks and benefits when making decisions (Baron, 1988; Weinstein and Fineberg, 1980). In addition, alcohol expectancy researchers have found that perceived benefits, in addition to perceived risks, are important predictors of drinking behavior (Christiansen, Goldman, and Inn, 1982; Christiansen, Roehling, Smith, and Goldman, 1989; Chen, Grube, and Madden, 1994; Goldberg et al., 2002; Grube, Chen, Madden, and Morgan, 1995; Jones, Corbin, and Fromme, 2001; Smith, Goldman, Greenbaum, and Christiansen, 1995; Wood, Nagoshi, and Dennis, 1992). More recently, Goldberg and colleagues (2002) found that, regardless of age, participants with more drinking experience perceived benefits to be more likely to occur, and risks less likely.

Indeed, adolescents’ reasons for drinking alcohol often include an acknowledgment or identification of alcohol-related benefits, such as alcohol being used in social interactions to help them to reduce inhibitions, feel more relaxed (Jones et al., 2001; Wood et al., 1992), reduce tension, foster courage, and reduce worry (Prendergast, 1994).

Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×

Age, Gender, and Experiential Differences in Alcohol-Related Expectancies

Both positive and negative alcohol-related expectancies vary by age. By age 12, individuals with and without drinking experience have a well-formulated sense of alcohol-related expectancies (Christiansen et al., 1982; Jones et al., 2001). Although children’s and adults’ general alcohol expectancies are similar (Dunn and Goldman, 1996), negative expectancies are more often reported by younger children, with perceptions of alcohol-related benefits increasing with age (Goldberg et al., 2002; Miller, Smith, and Goldman, 1990). Specific expectancies also differ by age. Young adolescents (e.g., 12- to 14-year-olds) rank reduced tension and impaired behavioral functioning among the highest expectancies; 15- to 16-year-olds cite enhanced social and physical pleasure and modified social and emotional behavior; and older adolescents (e.g., 17- to 19-year-olds) list enhanced sexual performance and increased power among their highest expectancies (Christiansen et al., 1982).

There are also gender differences in alcohol expectations. Adolescent males typically perceive more positive and fewer negative consequences of alcohol use than do adolescent females. Furthermore, the frequency of alcohol use has been associated with global positive effects, sexual enhancement, and pleasure for men, but reduced tension for women (Jones et al., 2001). However, there were no differences between males and females in the relationship between amount of alcohol use and perceived outcomes.

Independent of age effects, prior experience with alcohol use also plays a large role in alcohol-related expectancies, alcohol-related decision making, and subsequent alcohol use (Christiansen et al., 1989; Christiansen et al., 1982; Chen et al., 1994; Goldberg et al., 2002; Grube et al., 1995; Jones et al., 2001; Smith et al., 1995; Wood et al., 1992). In particular, enhanced sexual feelings, power, and reduced tension have been reported by those with greater drinking experiences, while youth with little or no alcohol experiences have more global expectancies of increased pleasure (Christiansen et al., 1982).

ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES ON UNDERAGE ALCOHOL USE

At the same time that youth are experiencing rapid physical, cognitive, psychosocial, and emotional changes, they are embedded within changing and multilayered contexts. These contexts and their reciprocal relationship with youth also must be understood in order to reduce underage drinking most effectively. In this section, we discuss the roles that parents, peers, and the larger society play in underage alcohol use.

Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×

Parents

Although multiple influences in adolescents’ lives will determine their developmental course, parents have the single most important external influence on adolescents’ development and behavior, including alcohol use. For more than 50 years, the relationship between parents’ child-rearing orientations and child outcomes has been studied. This venture has resulted in a host of literature linking parenting styles to substance use (e.g., Lamborn, Mounts, Steinberg, and Dornbusch, 1991). Typically, researchers have found that children raised in homes in which parents were warm, loving, and involved, supported their children’s independence and valued their children’s opinions, and also set limits and monitored their children’s activities were more likely to exhibit positive developmental outcomes and less likely to exhibit risk behaviors than were children reared in less supportive homes.

With respect to alcohol use, parental monitoring and involvement have emerged as particularly important parenting behaviors. Parental monitoring includes parental or guardian supervision and knowledge about their adolescents’ activities and friends. Parental monitoring also entails setting clear expectations and having fair and direct consequences (Connell, Halpern-Felsher, Clifford, Crichlow, and Usinger, 1995; Connell and Halpern-Felsher, 1997; Halpern-Felsher, et al., 1997; Lee and Halpern-Felsher, 2001). Correlated with parental monitoring is parental involvement, which includes the parents having an active interest and participation in their child’s activities and development. Parents can prevent or reduce adolescents’ alcohol use in part by setting curfews, being aware of and participating in after-school and weekend activities, helping them to improve social skills, and reducing their adolescents’ affiliation with peers who are engaging in risk activities (Beck and Lockhart, 1992; Cohen, Richardson, and LaBree, 1994; Steinberg, Fletcher, and Darling, 1994). Research on parental monitoring consistently shows protective effects on alcohol use (Barnes, Reifman, Farrell, and Dintcheff, 2000; Bogenschneider, Wu, Raffaelli, and Tsay, 1998; Reifman, Barnes, Dintcheff, Farrell, and Uhteg, 1998; DiClemente et al., 2001).

Parental norms and attitudes regarding adolescents’ alcohol use (Ary, Tildesley, Hops, and Andrews, 1993; Baer, Barr, Bookstein, Sampson, and Streissguth, 1998; McGue, Sharma, and Benson, 1996; Stice, Barrera, and Chassin, 1998; Webb and Baer, 1995; Weinberg, Dielman, Mandell, and Shope, 1994) are also important influences on use (e.g., Sieving, Perry, and Williams, 2000). Studies have also shown that parents’ own alcohol use is associated with underage drinking (e.g., Pandina and Johnson, 1989) as well as increased chance of experiencing alcohol-related negative consequences (Pandina and Johnson, 1990). Parents’ use of alcohol is related to

Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×

less family support, reduced parental monitoring, and less effective coping strategies, which in turn are related to a greater likelihood of alcohol use among adolescents (Barnes et al., 2000; e.g., Johnson and Pandina, 1993; Reifman et al., 1998).

Studies have also demonstrated that sibling alcohol use is a risk factor in a target individual’s alcohol use (Ary et al., 1993; McGue et al., 1996). For example, McGue and colleagues (1996) showed that while parental alcohol use only had an effect for the biological children, sibling alcohol use was related to both adoptive and biological children’s alcohol consumption. The effect was stronger if the sibling was similar in age, gender, and ethnicity.

Peers

Adolescents and adults spend a greater amount of time with their peers than with their family or other adults. Thus, peers become a major source of support and socialization for youth. Research shows that these peer networks can play a role in underage drinking. Adolescents are more likely to consume alcohol if they affiliate with peers who consume alcohol (Ary et al., 1993; Colder and Chassin, 1999; Fergusson, Horwood, and Lynskey, 1995; Curran, Stice, and Chassin, 1997; Dielman, Shope, Butchart, Campanelli, and Caspar, 1989; Sieving et al., 2000; Stice et al., 1998; Webster, Hunter, and Keats, 1994), have friends who offer them alcohol (Sieving et al., 2000), and are encouraged to use alcohol (Duncan, Duncan, and Hops, 1994; Keefe, 1994).

The potentially negative influence of peers on alcohol use can be ameliorated through positive adolescent-parent relationships, parental monitoring, and parental involvement (Parke and Ladd, 1992). For example, compared to less involved parents, parents who are more involved may oversee and monitor their children’s peer relationships. Parents also have a significant amount of influence on their children’s actual choice of friends, helping them to gravitate toward peers with fewer risk behaviors.

Several prevention efforts have attempted to alleviate the effects of peer influences on alcohol use. One area of programmatic focus has been to try to change perceptions of alcohol-related social norms. Such efforts have been based on research indicating that youth are aware of the normative nature of alcohol use and want to follow the crowd (Aas and Klepp, 1992; Barnes, Farrell, and Banerjee, 1995; Beck and Treiman, 1996; Olds and Thombs, 2001). Perceived use of alcohol by one’s peers and friends independently predicts self-reported alcohol use (e.g., Olds and Thombs, 2001; Reifman et al., 1998), with peers having a greater influence on adolescent drinking than parents (Kuther, 2002). However, such programmatic efforts should proceed cautiously given evidence that two types of norms must be

Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×

distinguished and acknowledged (Cialdini, Reno, and Kallgren, 1990; Kallgren, Reno, and Cialdini, 2000). One type, descriptive norms, are one’s perceptions of what most others are doing. The second type, injunctive norms, are the perceptions of what other people think one should be doing or not be doing. Cialdini and colleagues argue that focusing on injunctive norms is more effective at changing behavior than targeting only descriptive norms.

Another effort to reduce the influence of peer pressure has been to teach and encourage drinking refusal self-efficacy (e.g., Bell, Ellickson, and Harrison, 1993; Ellickson, Bell, and Harrison, 1993). This concept, borrowed from Bandura’s (1986, 1997) general self-efficacy, refers to one’s belief in his or her ability to resist urges or social pressures to drink, to drink in particular situations, or to consume large amounts of alcohol at one time. Adolescents with more positive self-efficacy are less likely to drink or drink excessively (Oei, Fergusson, and Lee, 1998; Webb and Baer, 1995), and those with fewer refusal skills are more likely to drink (Hays and Ellickson, 1996). Refusal skills may be a better predictor of problem drinking than alcohol expectancies, especially for heavy or frequent alcohol use (Connor, Young, Williams, and Ricciardelli, 2000; Oei et al., 1998).

Society and Community

We live in a society in which alcohol is ubiquitous, glamorized, and touted as a hallmark of adulthood. Advertisements for alcohol use are abundant, and alcohol is prominently displayed on television, in movies, in music, and at sports events. These societal messages compete with and even overshadow messages against underage drinking. Such mixed messages are particularly confusing to youth who are trying to make sense of their changing body and world, and who are trying to understand and meet societal expectations.

The extent to which alcohol consumption is considered to be the norm and is accepted within a given community contributes to variation in underage alcohol consumption. Underage drinking is less prevalent in communities in which youth are well monitored and where there is greater policing and enforcement of vendors who sell alcohol to youth. An especially vulnerable time in which youth particularly need supervision is after school (3 to 6 p.m.). Youth spend approximately 40 to 50 percent of their hours in discretionary time (Larson and Verma, 1999), with much of it unsupervised for older adolescents. Youth who participate in after-school programs, such as sports, clubs, library-based activities, and youth-serving organizations, are less likely to use alcohol (Eccles and Barber, 1999). Unfortunately, adolescents’ participation in these after-school programs has lessened over

Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×

the past two decades, largely because of reduced funding and because transportation and accessibility to these programs is often limited.

Colleges and universities are also a prime example of a community in which alcohol use is accepted and rampant. Despite college and university administration policies regarding underage drinking, the college environment continues to be a major time of alcohol use for youth. During college, underage youth can easily obtain alcohol from college mates older than age 21, and at times parents even supply college students with kegs of beer.

SUMMARY

Clearly, a great number of developmental and environmental factors contribute to underage drinking. All of these influences, from the most basic pubertal maturation to the most macrolevel societal policies, interact in complex ways. It must be recognized that behavior does not occur in a vacuum and cannot be understood adequately without considering the interplay between the individual and the many different types of environments in which he or she lives. For example, the confluence of cognitive and psychosocial maturation juxtaposed with societal expectations lead to shifts in youths’ focus of affiliation gradually from parents to peers and from group relations to intimate relations with individuals outside of the family. These expanding social relationships broaden adolescents’ sense of extra-familial reality and reinforce their increasing sense of individuality, need for autonomy, and desire to take on adult roles. Their newly acquired ability to think abstractly and to take a third person’s perspective are important prerequisites for successful socialization that is tied to new responsibilities and freedom. However, such developments may also lead to risky choices, such as alcohol consumption.

Although we cannot change development, we can understand it, and we can use knowledge about adolescent and young adult development to inform the creation of more effective intervention programs. Knowledge about the development of future perspective taking, for example, is important to the development of alcohol-related prevention and intervention programs. Programs that focus on long-term outcomes will be less applicable and less effective for a young adolescent of about age 12 for whom there is an immature sense of the future and future consequences.

In addition, we can focus efforts on the environmental influences and the confluence between factors that are predictors of alcohol use. For example, enhancing parental monitoring and involvement, whereby parents are more aware of their child’s whereabouts, is likely to result in youth who are less likely to use alcohol. Alcohol-related expectations are also related to alcohol use, with alcohol use positively related to perceived benefits and

Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×

negatively related to perceived risks. As pointed out by Hawkins, Catalano, and Miller (1992:87), prevention programs should include a “developmentally adjusted, multiple-component risk-reduction strategy that cuts across traditional health, education, and human service delivery systems.”

REFERENCES

Aas, H., and Klepp, K.I. (1992). Adolescents’ alcohol use related to perceived norms. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 33(4), 315-325.

Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behavior. In J. Kuhl, and J. Bechmann (Eds.), Action-control: From cognition to behavior (pp. 11-39). New York: Springer-Verlag.

Ary, D.V., Tildesley, E., Hops, H., and Andrews, J. (1993). The influence of parent, sibling, and peer modeling and attitudes on adolescent use of alcohol. International Journal of the Addictions, 28(9), 853-880.


Baer, J.S., Barr, H.M., Bookstein, F.L., Sampson, P.D., and Streissguth, A.P. (1998). Prenatal alcohol exposure and family history of alcoholism in the etiology of adolescent alcohol problems. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 59(5), 533-543.

Baltes, P.B. (1987). Theoretical propositions of lifespan developmental psychology: On the dynamics between growth and decline. Developmental Psychology, 23, 611-626.

Baltes, P.B., Reese, H.W., and Lipsitt, L.P. (1980). Lifespan developmental psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 31, 65-110.

Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Bandura, A. (1994). Social cognitive theory and exercise of control over HIV infection. In R.J. DiClemente and J.L. Peterson (Eds.), Preventing AIDS: Theories and methods of behavioral interventions (pp. 24-29). New York: Plenum Press.

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of self-control. New York: W.H. Freeman.

Barnes, G.M., Farrell, M.P., and Banerjee, S. (1995). Family influences on alcohol abuse and other problem behaviors among black and white Americans in a general population sample. In G.M. Boyd, J. Howard, and R.A. Zucker (Eds.), Alcohol problems among adolescents: Current directions in prevention research (pp. 13-31). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Barnes, G.M., Reifman, A.S., Farrell, M.P., and Dintcheff, B.A. (2000). The effects of parenting on the development of adolescent alcohol misuse: A six-wave latent growth model. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 62(1), 175-186.

Baron, J. (1988). Thinking and deciding. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Beck, K.H., and Lockhart, S.J. (1992). A model of parental involvement in adolescent drinking and driving. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 21, 35-51.

Beck, K.H., and Treiman, K.A. (1996). The relationship of social context of drinking, perceived social norms, and parental influence to various drinking patterns of adolescents. Addictive Behaviors, 21(5), 633-644.

Bell, R.M., Ellickson, P.L., and Harrison, E.R. (1993). Do drug prevention effects persist into high school? How Project ALERT did with ninth graders. Preventive Medicine, 22, 463-483.

Beyth-Marom, R., Austin, L., Fischhoff, B., Palmgren, C., and Jacobs-Quadrel, M. (1993). Perceived consequences of risky behaviors: Adults and adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 29, 549-563.

Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×

Bogenschneider, K., Wu, M.Y., Raffaelli, M., and Tsay, J.C. (1998). Parent influences on adolescent peer orientation and substance use: The interface of parenting practices and values. Child Development, 69, 1672-1688.


Chen, M.J., Grube, J.W., and Madden, P.A. (1994). Alcohol expectancies and adolescent drinking: Differential prediction of frequency, quantity, and intoxication. Addictive Behaviors, 19, 521-529.

Christiansen, B.A., Goldman, M.S., and Inn, A. (1982). Development of alcohol-related expectancies in adolescents: Separating pharmacological from social-learning influences. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50, 336-344.

Christiansen, B.A., Roehling, P., Smith, G., and Goldman, M. (1989). Using alcohol expectancies to predict adolescent drinking behavior after one year. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 93-99.

Cialdini, R.B., Reno, R.R., and Kallgren, C.A. (1990). A focus theory of normative conduct: Recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 1015-1026.

Cohen, D.A., Richardson, J., and LaBree, L. (1994). Parenting behaviors and the onset of smoking and alcohol use: A longitudinal study. Pediatrics, 94, 368-375.

Colder, C.R., and Chassin, L. (1999). The psychosocial characteristics of alcohol users versus problem users: Data from a study of adolescents at risk. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 321-348.

Connell, J.P., and Halpern-Felsher, B.L. (1997). How neighborhoods affect educational outcomes in middle childhood and adolescence: Conceptual issues and an empirical example. In J. Brooks-Gunn, G. Duncan, and J.L. Aber (Eds.), Neighborhood poverty, volume I: Context and consequences for children (pp. 174-199). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

Connell, J.P., Halpern-Felsher, B.L., Clifford, E., Crichlow, W., and Usinger, P. (1995). Hanging in there: Behavioral, psychological, and contextual factors affecting whether African-American adolescents stay in high school. Journal of Adolescent Research, 10, 41-63.

Connor, J.P., Young, R.M., Williams, R.J., and Ricciardelli, L.A. (2000). Drinking restraint versus alcohol expectancies: Which is the better indicator of alcohol problems? Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 61, 352-359.

Curran, P.J., Stice, E., and Chassin, L. (1997). The relation between adolescent alcohol use and peer alcohol use: A longitudinal random coefficients model. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 130-140.


De Rosenroll, D.A. (1987). Early adolescent egocentrism: A review of six articles. Adolescence, 22, 791-802.

DiClemente, R.J., Wingood, G.M., Crosby, R., Sionean, C., Cobb, B.K., Harrington, K., Davies, S., Hook, E.W., III, and Oh, M.K. (2001). Parental monitoring: Association with adolescents’ risk behaviors. Pediatrics, 107, 1363-1368.

Dielman, T.E., Butchart, A.T., Shope, J.T., and Miller, M. (1991). Environmental correlates of adolescent substance use and misuse—Implications for prevention programs. International Journal of the Addictions, 25(7A-8A), 855-880.

Dielman, T.E., Shope, J.T., Butchart, A.T., Campanelli, P.C., and Caspar, R.A. (1989). A covariance structure model test of antecedents of adolescent alcohol misuse and a prevention effort. Journal of Drug Education, 19, 337-361.

Dolcini, M.M., Cohn, L.D., Adler, N.E., Millstein, S.G., Irwin, C.E., Kegeles, S.M., and Stone, G.C. (1989). Adolescent egocentrism and feelings of invulnerability: Are they related? Journal of Early Adolescence, 9, 409-418.

Duncan, T.E., Duncan, S.C., and Hops, H. (1994). The effects of family cohesiveness and peer encouragement on the development of adolescent alcohol-use: A cohort-sequential approach to the analysis of longitudinal data. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 55, 588-599.

Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×

Dunn, M.E., and Goldman, M.S. (1996). Empirical modeling of an alcohol expectancy memory network in elementary school children as a function of grade. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 4, 209-217.


Eccles, J.S., and Barber, B.L. (1999). Student council, volunteering, basketball, or marching band: What kind of extracurricular involvement matters? Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 6, 281-294.

Elkind, D. (1967). Egocentrism in adolescence. Child Development, 38, 1025-1034.

Elkind, D. (1978). Understanding the young adolescent. Adolescence, 13, 127-134.

Ellickson, P.L., Bell, R.M., and Harrison, E.R. (1993). Changing adolescent propensities to use drugs: Results from Project ALERT. Health Education Quarterly, 20(2), 227-242.


Fergusson, D.M., Horwood, L.J., and Lynskey, M.T. (1995). The prevalence and risk-factors associated with abusive or hazardous alcohol-consumption in 16-year-olds. Addiction, 90, 935-946.

Fishbein, M., and Ajzen, I. (1975). Beliefs, attitudes, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.


Goldberg, J.H., Halpern-Felsher, B.L., and Millstein, S.G. (2002). Beyond invulnerability: The importance of benefits in adolescents’ decision to drink alcohol. Health Psychology, 21, 477-484.

Gray, W., and Hudson, L. (1984). Formal operations and the imaginary audience. Developmental Psychology, 20, 619-627.

Grube, J.W., Chen, M., Madden, P., and Morgan, M. (1995). Predicting adolescent drinking from alcohol expectancy values: A comparison of additive, interactive, and nonlinear models. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25, 839-857.


Halebsky, M.A. (1987). Adolescent alcohol and substance abuse: Parent and peer effects. Adolescence, 22, 961-967.

Halpern-Felsher, B.L., Connell, J.P., Spencer, M.B., Aber, J.L., Duncan, G.P., Clifford, E., Crichlow, W.E., Usinger, P.A., Cole, S.P., Allen, L., and Seidman, E. (1997). Neighborhood and family factors predicting educational risk and attainment in African American and white children and adolescents. In J. Brooks-Gunn, G. Duncan, and J.L. Aber (Eds.), Neighborhood poverty, volume I: Context and consequences for children (pp. 146-173). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

Halpern-Felsher, B.L., Millstein, S.G., Ellen, J., Adler, N., Tschann, J., and Biehl, M. (2001). The role of behavioral experience in judging risks. Health Psychology, 20, 120-126.

Hampson, S.E., Severson, H.H., Burns, W.J., Slovic, P., and Fisher, K.J. (2001). Risk perception, personality factors and alcohol use among adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 167-181.

Hawkins, J.D., Catalano, R.F., and Miller, J.Y. (1992). Risk and protective factors for alcohol and other drug problems in adolescence and early adulthood: Implications for substance-abuse prevention. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 64-105.

Hays, R.D., and Ellickson, P.L. (1996). What is adolescent alcohol misuse in the United States according to the experts? Alcohol and Alcoholism, 31, 297-303.

Hill, J.P. (1973). Some perspectives on adolescence in American society. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare Office of Child Development.


Jahnke, H.C., and Blanchard-Fields, F. (1993). A test of two models of adolescent egocentrism. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 22, 313-326.

Johnson, V., and Pandina, R.J. (1993). A longitudinal examination of the relationships among stress, coping strategies, and problems associated with alcohol use. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 17, 696-702.

Jones, B.T., Corbin, W., and Fromme, K. (2001). A review of expectancy theory and alcohol consumption. Addiction, 96, 427-436.

Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×

Kallgren, C.A., Reno, R.R., and Cialdini, R.B. (2000). A focus theory of normative conduct: When norms do and do not affect behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1002-1012.

Kanfer, F.H. (1970). Self-regulation: Research, issues and speculations. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Keefe, K. (1994). Perceptions of normative social pressure and attitudes toward alcohol-use: Changes during adolescence. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 55, 46-54.

Kuther, T.L. (2002). Rational decision perspectives on alcohol consumption by youth—Revising the theory of planned behavior. Addictive Behaviors, 27, 35-47.


Lamborn, S.D., Mounts, N.S., Steinberg, L., and Dornbusch, S.M. (1991). Patterns of competence and adjustment among adolescents from authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent, and neglectful families. Child Development, 62, 1049-1065.

Lapsley, D., Milstead, M., Quintana, S.M., Flannery, D., and Buss, R.R. (1986). Adolescent egocentrism and formal operations: Tests of a theoretical assumption. Developmental Psychology, 22, 800-807.

Larson, R.W., and Verma, S. (1999). How children and adolescents spend time across the world: Work, play, and developmental opportunities. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 701-736.

Lee, C.Y., and Halpern-Felsher, B.L. (2001). Parenting of adolescents. In L. Balter (Ed.), Parenthood in America: An Encyclopedia (Vol. 1, pp. 14-17). Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-CLIO.

Lerner, R.M., Miller, J.R., Knott, J.H., et al. (1994). Integrating scholarship and outreach in human development research, policy, and service: A developmental contextual perspec-tive. In D.L. Featherman, R.M. Lerner, and M. Perlmutter (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 12, pp. 249-273). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.


McGue, M., Sharma, A., and Benson, P. (1996). Parent and sibling influences on adolescent alcohol use and misuse: Evidence from a U.S. adoption cohort. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 57, 8-18.

Miller, P.M., Smith, G.T., and Goldman, M.S. (1990). Emergence of alcohol expectancies in childhood: A possible critical period. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 51, 343-349.


Oei, T.P., Fergusson, S., and Lee, N.K. (1998). The differential role of alcohol expectancies and drinking refusal self-efficacy in problem and nonproblem drinkers. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 59, 704-711.

Olds, R.S., and Thombs, D.L. (2001). The relationship of adolescent perceptions of peer norms and parent involvement to cigarette and alcohol use. Journal of School Health, 71, 223-228.


Pandina, R.J., and Johnson, V. (1989). Familial drinking history as a predictor of alcohol and drug consumption among adolescent children. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 50, 245-253.

Pandina, R.J., and Johnson, V. (1990). Serious alcohol and drug problems among adolescents with a family history of alcoholism. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 51, 278-282.

Parke, R.D., and Ladd, G.W. (Eds.). (1992). Family-peer relationships: Modes of linkage. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Prendergast, L. (1994). Substance use and abuse among college students: A review of the recent literature. Journal of American College Health, 43, 99-113.


Quadrel, M.J., Fischhoff, B., and Davis, W. (1993). Adolescent (in)vulnerability. American Psychologist, 48, 102-116.


Reifman, A., Barnes, G.M., Dintcheff, B.A., Farrell, M.P., and Uhteg, L. (1998). Parental and peer influences on the onset of heavier drinking among adolescents. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 59, 311-317.

Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×

Rosenstock, I.M. (1974). Historical origins of the health belief model. In M.H. Becker (Ed.), The health belief model and personal health behavior (pp. 1-8). Thorofare, NJ: Charles B. Sclack.


Sieving, R.E., Perry, C.L., and Williams, C.L. (2000). Do friendships change behaviors, or do behaviors change friendships? Examining paths of influence in young adolescents’ alcohol use. Journal of Adolescent Health, 26, 27-35.

Silverberg, S.B., and Gondoli, D.M. (1996). Autonomy in adolescence: A contextualized perspective. In G.R. Adams, R. Montemayor, and T.P. Gullotta (Eds.), Psychosocial development during adolescence: Progress in developmental contextualism (pp. 12-61). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Smith, G., Goldman, M., Greenbaum, P., and Christiansen, B. (1995). Expectancy for social facilitation from drinking: The divergent paths of high-expectancy and low-expectancy adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 32-40.

Steinberg, L., and Cauffman, E. (1996). Maturity of judgment in adolescence: Psychosocial factors in adolescent decision making. Law and Human Behavior, 20, 249-272.

Steinberg, L.D., Fletcher, A., and Darling, N. (1994). Parental monitoring and peer influences on adolescent substance use. Pediatrics, 93, 1060-1064.

Stice, E., Barrera, M., and Chassin, L. (1998). Prospective differential prediction of adolescent alcohol use and problem use: Examining the mechanisms of effect. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 616-628.

Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. (2001). National household survey on drug abuse. Rockville, MD: Author.


Triandis, H.C. (1977). Interpersonal behavior. Monterey, CA: Brooks Cole.


Urberg, K., and Robbins, R. (1984). Perceived vulnerability in adolescents to the health consequences of cigarette smoking. Preventive Medicine, 13, 367-376.


Vakalahi, H.F. (2001). Adolescent substance use and family-based risk and protective factors: A literature review. Journal of Drug Education, 31, 29-46.


Webb, J.A., and Baer, P.R. (1995). Influence of family disharmony and parental alcohol-use on adolescent social skills, self-efficacy, and alcohol-use. Addictive Behaviors, 20, 127-135.

Webster, R.A., Hunter, M., and Keats, J.A. (1994). Personality and sociodemographic influences on adolescents’ substance use: A path-analysis. International Journal of the Addictions, 29, 941-956.

Weinberg, N.Z., Dielman, T.E., Mandell, W., and Shope, J.T. (1994). Parental drinking and gender factors in the prediction of early adolescent alcohol-use. International Journal of the Addictions, 29, 89-104.

Weinstein, M., and Fineberg, H. (1980). Clinical decision analysis. Philadelphia: Saunders.

Weinstein, N.D. (1983). Reducing unrealistic optimism about illness. Health Psychology, 2, 11-20.

Weisenberg, M., Kegeles, S.S., and Lund, A.K. (1980). Children’s health beliefs and acceptance of a dental preventive activity. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 21, 59-74.

Wood, M.D., Nagoshi, C.T., and Dennis, D.A. (1992). Alcohol norms and expectations as predictors of alcohol use and problems in a college student sample. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 18, 461-476.

Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×
Page 402
Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×
Page 403
Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×
Page 404
Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×
Page 405
Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×
Page 406
Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×
Page 407
Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×
Page 408
Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×
Page 409
Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×
Page 410
Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×
Page 411
Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×
Page 412
Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×
Page 413
Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×
Page 414
Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×
Page 415
Suggested Citation:"4 Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview--Bonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl." National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2004. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/10729.
×
Page 416
Next: 5 Perceptions of Risk and Social Judgments: Biases and Motivational Factors--Janis E. Jacobs »
Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility Get This Book
×
Buy Hardback | $49.95 Buy Ebook | $39.99
MyNAP members save 10% online.
Login or Register to save!
Download Free PDF

Alcohol use by young people is extremely dangerous - both to themselves and society at large. Underage alcohol use is associated with traffic fatalities, violence, unsafe sex, suicide, educational failure, and other problem behaviors that diminish the prospects of future success, as well as health risks – and the earlier teens start drinking, the greater the danger. Despite these serious concerns, the media continues to make drinking look attractive to youth, and it remains possible and even easy for teenagers to get access to alcohol.

Why is this dangerous behavior so pervasive? What can be done to prevent it? What will work and who is responsible for making sure it happens? Reducing Underage Drinking addresses these questions and proposes a new way to combat underage alcohol use. It explores the ways in which may different individuals and groups contribute to the problem and how they can be enlisted to prevent it. Reducing Underage Drinking will serve as both a game plan and a call to arms for anyone with an investment in youth health and safety.

  1. ×

    Welcome to OpenBook!

    You're looking at OpenBook, NAP.edu's online reading room since 1999. Based on feedback from you, our users, we've made some improvements that make it easier than ever to read thousands of publications on our website.

    Do you want to take a quick tour of the OpenBook's features?

    No Thanks Take a Tour »
  2. ×

    Show this book's table of contents, where you can jump to any chapter by name.

    « Back Next »
  3. ×

    ...or use these buttons to go back to the previous chapter or skip to the next one.

    « Back Next »
  4. ×

    Jump up to the previous page or down to the next one. Also, you can type in a page number and press Enter to go directly to that page in the book.

    « Back Next »
  5. ×

    Switch between the Original Pages, where you can read the report as it appeared in print, and Text Pages for the web version, where you can highlight and search the text.

    « Back Next »
  6. ×

    To search the entire text of this book, type in your search term here and press Enter.

    « Back Next »
  7. ×

    Share a link to this book page on your preferred social network or via email.

    « Back Next »
  8. ×

    View our suggested citation for this chapter.

    « Back Next »
  9. ×

    Ready to take your reading offline? Click here to buy this book in print or download it as a free PDF, if available.

    « Back Next »
Stay Connected!