Below is the uncorrected machine-read text of this chapter, intended to provide our own search engines and external engines with highly rich, chapter-representative searchable text of each book. Because it is UNCORRECTED material, please consider the following text as a useful but insufficient proxy for the authoritative book pages.
5. Minoritized Individuals in STEMM: Consequences and Responses to Racial Bias and How STEMM Professionals Can Help The primary focus of the report so far has been on the experience of minoritized individuals in STEMM environments, situated within the larger sociopolitical context of the United States. As discussed in the first four chapters, the evidence has demonstrated that minoritized individuals have faced numerous systemic barriers, including macro-level policies and practices that have negatively impacted their opportunities, representation, and ability to thrive in STEMM careers. This chapter shifts the lens of analysis from an examination of racial bias at the systemic level to an examination of racial bias as it occurs at the individual and interpersonal level. In this, the present chapter addresses the charge in the statement of task on reviewing the research and evidence on the ways in which racism at the individual level impedes STEMM careers for minoritized individuals. Furthermore, this chapter also addresses the charge to identify methods of improving recruitment, retention, and advancement of members of minoritized groups. To do so, the committee reviews literature covering Black individuals, but also Latine and Indigenous individuals. The chapter begins by summarizing research describing how and why race and ethnicity are salient social categories. This research finds that as a result of several social cognitive processes, individuals create social categories, distinguish ingroups and outgroups, and assign personal meaning on the basis of race and ethnicity. All of these factors have implications for the ways in which individuals express and experience racial bias, and the consequences of racial bias are reviewed in detail in the second half of this section. These include a range of cognitive, emotional, and physiological outcomes associated with experiencing racism in STEMM contexts: negative impacts of predominately White contexts, stereotype threat and social identity threat, imposter phenomenon, stigma and coping, attributional ambiguity, physical health impacts, and race-based rejection sensitivity. This section concludes with a major key finding: 5-1 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
minoritized individuals experience a range of adverse cognitive, emotional, and physiological consequences as a result of facing racial bias. The second part of the chapter summarizes the noteworthy ways in which these individuals respond to bias in STEMM environments, including other professional spaces. These responses can be categorized into three general groups: exiting the field, implementing strategies to fit in, and collectively mobilizing to transform the STEMM environment. Each response has different implications for the advancement, recruitment, and retention of minoritized individuals in STEMM. The chapter concludes with a discussion of ways in which STEMM professionals can redesign local STEMM environments to support minoritized individuals. These strategies center on fostering connections, emphasizing communal values, affirming kindness cues, and increasing a sense of belonging. Overall, the committee focuses on the experience of being in minoritized groups that deviate from White norms to the extent that individuals in those groups are often the target of racial biases and systemic racism in the United Statesâpeople who are Black, Indigenous, and Latineâand the impact on individuals in STEMM contexts specifically. Broadly speaking, these minoritized groups control fewer resources, have less access to STEMM professions and fields, and are more vulnerable to racism and exclusion. The material in this chapter dovetails with that in Chapter 6, which turns from peopleâs experiences as the targets of racism to experiences of gatekeepers who possess power within STEMM and, whether consciously or unconsciously, perpetuate racism. SOCIAL COGNITIVE PROCESSES AND CONSEQUENCES OF RACIAL BIAS People carve the world into social categories or groups along many axes, including race and ethnicity, a tendency that can be seen in the social cognitive process of social categorization. The first part of this section details how creating such groups can lead to psychological experiences of âusâ and âthem.â This includes a consideration of how individuals may be members in particular social groups and subjectively identify with their groups to varying degrees (social identity), which has consequences for well-being when group esteem is threatened. The second part of the section demonstrates that there are a range of adverse 5-2 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
cognitive, emotional, and physiological consequences that minoritized individuals experience as a result of facing bias against their racial or ethnic group. SOCIAL CATEGORIZATION AND SOCIAL IDENTITY The human mind simplifies information processing through social categorization, which is the process of categorizing people into groups based on perceived visual, functional, or role similarity and inferring that members of a group share these and other common attributes (Fiske, 2005; Hilton & von Hippel, 1996; Macrae & Bodenhausen, 2000). Such inferences allow humans to take mental shortcuts in processing information about new people; thus, when a person encounters an individual for the first time, they often categorize that individual into a social group, based on perceived salient characteristics. This grouping is often accompanied by the inference that members of a given group are more similar to others in that group as compared to members of other groups. This kind of inference is the essence of stereotyping, which is the overgeneralized attribution of characteristics to others based on their group membership (Fiske, 2005) (see Chapter 6 for more on how social categorization underpins stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination). This process of social categorization is also reflexive, where individuals also think of themselves as belonging to groups. Groups to which an individual belongs are their âingroupâ whereas other groups to which they do not belong are their âoutgroup.â Thus, from the perspective of any given person, social categorization divides the world into âusâ (ingroups) and âthemâ (outgroups). Ingroups may be based on ascribed characteristics (e.g., groups based on age, sex, race, or ethnicity) or chosen characteristics (e.g., groups based on professions, roles, religion, nationality) (Brewer, 2007). Research using social identity theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1979; Tajfel, 1978) shows that a personâs identification with their various ingroups is a central component of that individualâs self-concept. Some ingroups may be more important to a personâs sense of self than others, in that people derive greater self-esteem from those identities (Brown, 2000). In addition, people exhibit a strong tendency to favor their ingroups relative to outgroups in social evaluations and resource allocations. This tendency has been used to explain intergroup prejudice as well as discriminatory behaviors (Böhm, Rusch, & Baron, 2020; Leaper, 2011; Wolfe & Spencer 1996). These phenomena are not limited to racial groups in United States, but they occur with various types of groups, everywhere, throughout history. 5-3 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
When particular groups are of high-status and power, whether historically or in the short- term as the result of specific situations, other groups are often measured against the norms established by the high-status group (Fiske et al., 2016). In relation to race, research shows that the more a person deviates from the non-Hispanic White norms (hereafter referred to as White) â a high-status groupâin terms of skin color, hair, accent, and physical features, the more racism a person is likely to experience (Blair, Judd, & Chapleau, 2004; Dixon & Telles, 2017; Eberhardt et al., 2006; Maddox & Perry, 2018) (see Chapter 2 for more on historical context for race and status in the United States). Below, this chapter describes a range of cognitive, emotional, and physiological outcomes associated with experiencing racism in STEMM contexts. This includes sections on the negative impacts of predominantly White contexts, stereotype threat and social identity threat, imposter phenomenon, stigma and coping, attributional ambiguity, physical health impacts, and race-based rejection sensitivity. Negative Impacts of Predominantly White Contexts For minoritized individuals in STEMM, navigating classrooms and environments in which they are a clear numeric minority has measurable psychological, physiological, and behavioral effects (see Chapter 3 for a summary of issues that contribute to numeric representation). These predominantly White contexts threaten two fundamental human needs. The first is the human need to belong. Anthropologists, social psychologists, and sociologists show that all humans have a universal need for attachment affiliation (Bowlby, 1969, 1973; Casella & Fowler, 2005; Cosmides et al., 1992; Maslow 1943, 1968; Sherif et al., 1988). From an evolutionary perspective, seeking belonging is necessary for survival across many species (De Waal, 1990, 2009). Affiliation needs lead humans to set aside personal desires for the sake of belonging to a group where roles, norms, and responsibilities promote the survival of all group- affiliated members (Cheney et al., 1986; Panter-Brick et al., 2001). Investigators have (Baumeister and Leary, 1995) concluded that âthe desire for interpersonal attachmentsâthe need to belongâis a fundamental human motivation.â People experience well-being and prosperity in the presence of social connection (Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Dunbar & Barrett, 2007) and stress and pain akin to physical pain in the presence of social exclusion (Eisenberger & Lieberman, 2005; Fiske, 2009). 5-4 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
The second fundamental need threatened by predominantly White contexts is the need to be valued and treated with dignity. Drawing on evolutionary, social, and developmental psychology, one scholar (Hicks, 2011) concluded that individuals feel their dignity is violated when some aspect of their identity is rejected. Dignity violation, which may include obvious acts of discrimination or subtle acts of ostracism, activates negative emotions that compromise decision making, social judgments, and problem-solving (Isen, 2008; Keltner & Lerner, 2010). Dignity violations in the context of STEMM academic settings impact cognitive functioning related to academic success, motivation, and an array of other outcomes to be described in the upcoming paragraphs. There are four features of predominantly White contexts that often make it difficult, if not impossible, for minoritized individuals to have these needs of belonging and dignity met in a full or satisfactory way. The first is that in such environments, there are few, if any, people who share oneâs identity; this results in a lack of high-status role models (Dasgupta, 2011; Griffith & Dasgupta, 2018; Sekaquaptewa et al., 2007; Thompson & Sekaquaptewa, 2002; Stout et al., 2011). The second is a lack of support and opportunity for building relationships with peers from similar backgrounds, which results in a weaker network (Blake-Beard et al., 2011; Dasgupta, 2011; Dennehy & Dasgupta, 2017; Thiem & Dasgupta, 2022). The third is inadequate access to material and knowledge resources resulting, in part, to resource-poor social networks and disconnection from central individuals with influence (Castilla et al., 2013a, 2013b; Jack, 2016, 2019; Mishra, 2020; Simmons, 2011; Stephens et al., 2012; Thiem & Dasgupta, 2022). The final feature is that in such environments, oneâs contributions are not recognized or valued (Castilla, 2008; Griffith et al., 2022; Misra et al., 2017). These four features are very much part of STEMM training and work environments, and as elsewhere, they have a profound impact on minoritized individuals who work in these types of local environments. They contribute to minoritized individuals feeling like solos or tokens, cut off from insider knowledge networks, and that their work and contribution is invisible (see Chapter 4 for descriptions of these phenomena from lived experience). As a result, individuals start to doubt their belonging, ability, and commitment to the endeavor, especially when alternative choices and paths are available (Dennehy et al., 2017; Thiem & Dasgupta, 2022). Threats to belonging that are driven by low numeric representation of similar others and absence of strong relationships or a social network are particularly potent in transition periods such as the 5-5 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
transition to college, to graduate school, or to a new job (Dasgupta, 2011; Hurtado & Carter, 1997; Thiem & Dasgupta, 2022). Stereotype Threat and Social Identity Threat Stereotype threat is situation-induced worry that arises in achievement-oriented situations when individuals feel apprehensive that their performance will be judged in ways that confirm negative stereotypes targeting their ingroup (Steele, 1997, 1998, 2011). This worry acts as a stressor for the individual, taxing their working memory capacity and undermining their objective performance relative to non-stereotyped group members. However, when an achievement-oriented situation is redesigned to explicitly disavow the negative stereotype (and be identity safe), the same individuals perform as well as non-stereotyped peers (for reviews, see Aronson & Steele, 2005; Crocker, Major, & Steele, 1998; Schmader, John, & Forbes, 2008; Steele, Spencer, & Aronson, 2002). A concept closely related to stereotype threat is social identity threat, a concept focusing on situations where individuals feel their valued identities are marginalized or ignored, which in turn affects much more than performanceâtheir sense of belonging, motivation, interests, persistence, and aspirations to persist. By activating negative stereotypes and undermining belonging, stereotype threat and social identity threat reduce self-confidence in oneâs ability (or self-efficacy) and leads individuals to withdraw from the domain. This is the experience of many minoritized students at predominantly White universities, women (of multiple racial and ethnic identities) in science and engineering, and professionals on upward career trajectories in professions where they (minoritized individuals, and women of multiple racial/ethnic identities) are often tokens (e.g., STEMM; Blascovich et al., 2001; Emerson & Murphy, 2014; Fischer, 2010; Lewis & Sekaquaptewa, 2016; Sekaquaptewa, Waldman, & Thompson, 2007; Spencer, Logel, & Davies, 2016; Steele, Spencer & Aronson, 2002). Consequently, many of these individuals tend to leave their academic or professional path (Ceci & Williams, 2010; Ceci, Williams, & Barnett, 2009; McArdle, 2008). Imposter Phenomenon Social identity threat sometimes leads individuals to lose confidence in their abilities, Consequently, they remove themselves from achievement-oriented environments even when their performance is equal to that of their peers. In other words, high performance and high 5-6 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
confidence are not always strongly correlated (Dasgupta, 2011; Stout et al., 2011). Thus, social identity threat is closely related to the imposter phenomenon, a term coined more than three decades ago (Clance & Imes, 1978). Research on the imposter phenomenon (also known as imposter syndrome) shows that sometimes individuals privately believe they are faking talent (Clance & Imes, 1978; McGregor, Gee, & Posey, 2008); in these cases, they are more likely to attribute their strong performance to luck, effort, or personal charm (Chae et al.,1995; Clance, 1985; Thompson, Davis, & Davidson, 1998) instead of personal ability (Topping & Kimmel, 1985). They are dissatisfied with their performance even when it is at par with peers and colleagues, feel unsure of their ability, and have low expectations of repeated future success (Chrisman et al., 1995; Cozzarelli & Major, 1990; Kumar & Jagacinski, 2006; Thompson et al., 1998). Members of minoritized groups who are solos or tokens in high-achieving, predominately White contexts are particularly vulnerable to imposter phenomenon (Bravata et al., 2020). For example, African American graduate students at predominantly White universities experience imposter fear, which is associated with lower academic confidence or self-efficacy (Ewing et al., 1996). Likewise, for women who are a small numeric minority in engineering, academic failures are associated with a sharp decrease in self-esteem, and academic successes do not result in an increase in self-esteem (Crocker et al., 2003). Taken together, research demonstrates that the experience of being a numeric minority in high stakes achievement environmentsâlike those of STEMMâshakes individualsâ confidence in their own ability, especially in the face of difficulty. This remains true even if their actual performance is objectively equivalent to individuals of the majority group. Stigma and Coping Social identity threat is related to another body of research on stigma. A stigma is an attribute of a person that is associated with devalued stereotypes and is deeply discrediting in society (Goffman, 1963, p.3). A discredited attribute could be visible, such as oneâs skin color or body size, or could be hidden but discreditable if revealed, such as oneâs criminal record, struggles with mental illness, or poverty. Carrying a stigma complicates everyday interactionsâ stigmatized individuals may be wary of engaging with people who do not share their stigma; meanwhile, those without a certain stigma may disparage, overcompensate, or attempt to ignore stigmatized individuals. Research demonstrates the negative impacts of stigma on self-esteem, achievement, mental health, and physical well-being (for a review, see Major & OâBrien 2005; 5-7 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
also see Bryant-Davis & Ocampo 2005). Sometimes, societal stigmas are internalized by minoritized individuals, leading them to experience a lack of deservingness, self-directed racism, and self-devaluation (Frost 2011; Wester, Vogel, Wei, & McLain, 2011). In the face of stigma, individuals may engage in multiple forms of coping to regulate their emotion, cognition, and behavior (Sanchez, Smith & Adams, 2018). Some coping strategies help individuals buffer against the stressful event (Aspinwall, 2004), while others may exacerbate the stress. One review highlighted commonly used strategies for coping in the face of discrimination, such as attributing negative events to experiences of discrimination as opposed to internalizing the event and blaming oneself; disengaging self-esteem and effort from identity- threatening situations; and increasing oneâs personal identification with the stigmatized group (Major & OâBrien 2005). Attributional Ambiguity Even when situations do not activate social identity threat specifically, they often create a feeling of uncertainty in minoritized individuals about the cause of someone elseâs behavior toward them, be it positive or negative (Mendes et al., 2008; Major, Feinstein & Crocker 1994; Smith & Wout 2019). This feeling of uncertainty is called attributional ambiguity, and it happens in many contexts, including STEMM environments (Bandyopadhyay et al., 2022; OâBrien et al., 2016). Experiences of attributional ambiguity arise in interactions between people of different status groups, where the lower-status interaction partner feels uncertain about whether the cause of the higher-status personâs behavior toward them is based on personal deservingness (e.g., their actual ability or qualifications) or the result of racial bias. In the context of race, this ambiguity occurs when minoritized individuals interact with White individuals; in such interactions, the minoritized individual may wonder if positive feedback from the While individual is genuine or the result of overcompensating, and if negative feedback is deserved or the result of stereotyping or prejudice (Major, Quinton & McCoy, 2002). Evidence demonstrates that in interracial interactions (i.e., interactions between individuals of different races), Black individuals trust feedback more (both positive and negative) if their identity is masked than if their identity is revealed (Crocker et al., 1991). When their identity is revealed, they attribute negative feedback to the evaluatorâs prejudice, which protects their self-esteem, but they are unable to trust positive feedback as genuine, which undermines self-esteem. Individuals who belong to multiple minoritized groups, such as Black 5-8 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
women, may face additional obstacles of determining whether another personâs behavior toward them is a result of one of several types of bias (e.g., racism, sexism), a unique intersectional form of bias, or personal deservingness. Such intersectional forms of attributional ambiguity are under-researched (Remedios & Snyder 2015). Attributional ambiguity can yield both positive and negative outcomes for the minoritized individual (Aronson & Inzlicht, 2004; Hoyt et al., 2007; King 2003; Major & Crocker 1993). Attributing an ambiguous event to prejudice or discrimination may protect minoritized individualsâ self-esteem by preventing them from attributing the event to their personal characteristics (e.g., Crocker et al., 1991; Hoyt et al., 2007; Mendes et al., 2008). However, attributing events to discrimination may yield adverse outcomes as well, such as distress, reduced well-being, and threats to identity and belongingness (Albuja, Sanchez & Gaither, 2019; Chae, Lincoln, & Jackson, 2011; Miles et al., 2020). For instance, one study analyzed the perceptions of racial microaggressionsâsubtle or indirect form of racial discrimination (see Chapter 6 for more on microaggressions)âamong Black doctoral students in STEM and found that participantsâ sense of belonging and identity as scientists were negatively impacted (Miles et al., 2020). Mixed signals wherein minoritized individuals receive university communications that affirm commitment to diversity while simultaneously experiencing microaggressions in classroom settings may also contribute to attributional ambiguity in STEMM educational contexts (Estrada, Eroy-Reveles, & Matsui, 2018). PHYSICAL HEALTH IMPACTS OF BEING THE TARGET OF RACISM Over the past several years, there has been a substantial growth in research on the association between racism and health outcomes. This work shows that racism experienced at every level (e.g., institutional, interpersonal, etc.) undermines psychological health (Araújo & Borrell, 2006; Jones & Neblett, 2019; Pieterse et al., 2012) and physical health (Paradies et al., 2015; Solomon et al., 2022; Williams, Lawrence, & Davis, 2019; Williams et al., 2019). In fact, the director of the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) and leading medical societies, including American Medical Association (AMA), declared racism to be a serious public health threat (CDC, 2021; OâReilly, 2020). In terms of psychological health, racism faced by minoritized individuals is associated with increased anxiety (Stein et al., 2019), depressive symptoms (English et al., 2014), major depressive disorder (Russell et al., 2018), distress 5-9 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
(Nguyen, Cho, & Jang, 2021), and suicidal ideation (Madubata et al., 2022). Some scholars note that negative psychological consequences associated with racism share features associated with trauma (Pieterse et al., 2012). In terms of physical or physiological health, experiences of racism are significantly associated with poor cardiovascular health (Javed et al., 2022), hazardous drinking behavior (Gilbert & Zemore, 2016), poor sleep (Slopen, Lewis, & Williams, 2016), and cancer-related risks (Black, Johnson, & VanHoose, 2015; Plascak et al., 2022). A number of studies suggest that stress-induced âweatheringâ among older Black individuals, defined as the cumulative health impact of repeated experiences of social, economic, or political exclusion and effortful coping, induces morbidity both directly and indirectly through unhealthy behaviors (Geronimus, 2001; Geronimus et al., 2006). Weathering is thought to be partially responsible for race disparities in metabolic illnesses such as diabetes and cardiovascular disease. Multiple studies connect extended psychosocial stress to inflammation (McDade, Hawkley, & Cacioppo, 2006; Melamed, Shirom, Toker, Berliner, & Shapira, 2006; Weinstein, Vaupel, & Wachter, 2007). Inflammation, in turn, plays a causal role in cardiovascular problems and poor blood sugar control resulting from insulin resistance (Grundy et al., 2004; Yudkin, 2003). Chronic low-grade inflammation is linked to cardiovascular disease (Danesh et al., 2000), type 2 diabetes (Pradhan, Manson, Rifai, Buring, & Ridker, 2001) and metabolic syndrome (McDade & Hayward, 2009; Ridker, Buring, Cook, & Rifai, 2003). Using a nationally representative probability sample from the 2005â2006 U.S. National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project, one study found that inflammation was most consistently associated with older Black menâs greater metabolic problems, less control over blood sugar levels, and negative cardiovascular outcomes (Das, 2013). Moreover, inflammation significantly mediated menâs race disparities in metabolic states. The authors of that study argued that inflammation is an underexamined âbiological gatewayâ through which stressors in social environments affect older Black menâs diabetic and cardiovascular outcomes. In sum, the scientific literature shows that racism creates a substantial and disproportionate burden of illness on minoritized individuals. RACE-BASED REJECTION SENSITIVITY Research shows not all minoritized individuals experience the same context in the same way. For example, individual differences in expectations shape how minoritized individuals 5-10 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
interpret their environment, which in turn influences their adjustment to academia and other STEMM contexts. Some people are more sensitive to rejection based on their race, which influences their interpersonal experiences in predominantly White contexts. Research has found that college studentsâ expectations of race-based rejection can strain their social relationships and undermine their sense of confidence in the academic institution they are enrolled in. This has been found to result in reduced motivation to pursue personal goals among rejection-sensitive students, compared to less rejection-sensitive students who continued to persist (Mendoza- Denton et al., 2002). A later study showed that ethnic identification and race-based rejection sensitivity predicted decreased intention to persist in school among African Americans but did not predict lower grade point average (Mendoza-Denton, Pietrzak, & Downey, 2008). These findings demonstrate that individuals who belong to a given minoritized racial or ethnic group do not represent a monolith. There are important individual-level differences within members of a given group, and these differences can inform experiences and outcomes. Therefore, some minoritized individuals may thrive in predominately White contexts while others struggle. Additional research studying these individual-level differences is necessary and important for better understanding the discussed range of adverse cognitive, emotional, and physiological consequences. Summary The committee wishes to highlight a key finding that emerged from the information summarized in the above sections: Minoritized individuals experience a range of adverse cognitive, emotional, and physiological consequences as a result of facing racial bias. Moreover, individuals who occupy multiple minoritized identities may face added complexities and consequences navigating STEMM spaces as a result of facing multiple sources of bias. There is a paucity of research on the experience of such individuals, and the committee noted that more is needed. The committee also felt it important to emphasize that minoritized individuals in STEMM are not a monolith, both within a given group and between groups. As such, the experiences and consequences of racial bias may vary at the individual-level. Additional research and expanded sampling to include larger and more representative samples of all minoritized racial and ethnic groups is necessary to better understand these potential similarities and differences. 5-11 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
Minoritized Individualsâ Responses to Racial Bias The following section reviews three noteworthy ways in which minoritized individuals respond to experiences of racism. These responses can be grouped loosely into three groups: exiting the field, implementing strategies to fit in, and collectively mobilizing to transform the environment (see Figure 5-1; Brown, 1986). FIGURE 5-1 Minoritized individualsâ responses to facing racial bias. SOURCE: (Brown, 1986). STRATEGIES FOR EXITING: DISIDENTIFICATION AND PASSING Exiting is a common response for minoritized individuals experiencing racism in STEMM environments. Exiting generally encompasses two patterns: moving away from and often leaving the field (disidentification) and moving away from oneâs ingroup (passing) (Woodcock et al., 2012). National data of more than 200,000 students who started college in fall 2004 at 326 four-year institutions across the United States show that Black, Latine, and Native 5-12 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
American students enter college with aspirations to major in STEM at levels similar to their White and Asian peers. However, their paths diverge in college. Four-year (2008) and five-year (2009) college graduation rates reveal that Black, Latine, and Native American students complete college with STEM majors at lower rates (15.9, 13.2, and 14.0 percent, respectively) compared to their White and Asian American peers (24.5 and 32.4 percent, respectively). These differences become more pronounced when comparing the five-year graduation rate (Higher Education Research Institute, 2010) (see Chapter 3 on related data). In an attempt to identify specific barriers responsible for persistence gaps, other research identified a few key characteristics that differentiated students who earned bachelorâs degrees in STEM fields from those who did not (American Council on Education 2005; NASEM, 2011). This research found that students who successfully completed bachelorâs degrees in STEM were more likely to: (i) enter college with rigorous high school preparation; (ii) have access to high quality information and support related to higher education and career pathways that influenced their choice of college and navigation while in college; and (iii) have financial support that allowed them to focus on academic work without juggling long hours of paid work. In other words, high school preparation, social support, and financial capital strengthened studentsâ skills and persistence in STEM. Furthermore, there is evidence demonstrating that even after statistically accounting for studentsâ socioeconomic status and academic preparation, cultural experiences in college influenced STEM studentsâ performance, engagement, and persistence (Chang et al., 2011). Therefore, the nature of the context is another critical factor, and environments that fail to affirm inclusion may result in individuals implementing exiting strategies. These strategies are discussed next. Disidentification is one exiting strategy that minoritized individuals may use to deal with persistent social identity threat and belonging uncertainty. Disidentification is the process of becoming less invested in, or the long-term abandoning of, a formerly valued activity, interest, or social identity. Consequently, individuals may disidentify from their STEMM degree or profession, which places minoritized individuals at risk of leaving the field of STEMM (Woodcock et al., 2012). Ironically, the more a personâs sense of self is contingent upon doing well in a particular domain, the more vulnerable they are to social identity threat because of the worry that their underperformance or failure may prove the stereotype true in othersâ eyes. Disidentification 5-13 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
creates distance between the self and a domain that includes sets of specific stereotypes (e.g., STEMM), and thus is one way that minoritized individuals can mitigate the effects of social identity threat (Major & Schmader, 1998). While disidentifying from the field is an individual- level strategy implemented by minoritized individuals in the face of racism, larger racist systemic level barriers can continue to remain in place even after the person exits, if no top-down changes are made (see Chapter 8). Research shows that disidentification is more likely to happen in early stages of academic or professional development, such as when individuals in transition (e.g., college to a job), or among individuals who are new to the environment (Callagher et al., 2021). Consider Black, Indigenous, and Latine students at predominantly White universities or girls and women in science and engineering, where their ingroup is numerically underrepresented. Research has found there is a substantial number of individuals who are high performing who are exiting their field or taking a different academic or professional path (Ceci &Williams, 2010; Ceci, Williams, & Barnett, 2009; McArdle, 2008; Pinker, 2008; Rosenbloom et al., 2008). While it may appear that the choice to leave or opt for a different path appears to be made freely, there is a body of research demonstrating that these critical decisions are in fact shaped by stereotypes. These stereotypes inform ideas about who is likely to succeed or fail and who belongs in a given environment (Cheryan et al., 2009; Logel et al., 2009; Settles, 2004; Spencer, Steele, & Quinn, 1999; Steele, James, & Barnett, 2002; Stout et al., 2011; von Hippel et al., 2011; Walton & Cohen, 2007, 2011; Woodcock et al., 2012). For example, a three-year longitudinal study of African American and Hispanic/Latine undergraduate students in STEM majors showed that experiences of stereotype threat were associated with scientific disidentification, which in turn predicted a significant decline in the intention to pursue a scientific career (Woodcock et al., 2012). This effect was stronger for Hispanic/Latine students than African American students. Other research found that African Americans experiencing stereotype threat disengaged from academics in response to negative performance feedback (Nussbaum & Steele, 2007). Passingâpsychologically distancing oneself from oneâs ascribed social ingroupâis another type of exiting. Passing is only possible if a person can conceal their disadvantaged group membership, which may be the case for individuals from poor or working-class origins, individuals who are lesbian, gay, bisexual, or transgender, and Black, Indigenous, and Latine 5-14 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
people with light skin color who can pass as White. This is a form of exiting as well, wherein an individual âexitsâ their own social identity and no long thinks of themselves as a member of the racial or ethnic group (Brown, 1986). There is a small set of contemporary research on passing among minoritized individuals, particularly in the context of STEMM (McGee, 2016; Ong, 2005). Additional research is necessary to better understand this phenomenon. STRATEGIES FOR FITTING IN AND SURVIVING: GRIT, RESILIENCE, AND CODESWITCHING Grit and resilience are related concepts that describe how minoritized individuals might try to fit in and survive in achievement-oriented environments, such as STEMM. Grit is an individual trait that reflects a single-minded pursuit of long-term goals regardless of setbacks (Duckworth et al., 2007). Studies show that grit is significantly associated with improved academic performance (Christopoulou et al., 2018). Resilience is defined as a healthy and adaptive functioning following adverse events (Southwick et al., 2014). It is an individualâs ability to bounce back or overcome difficult stressors and adversity (Masten & Narayan, 2012). Resilience is sometimes conceptualized as an individual-level trait (Debb et al., 2018) and at other times as a process of adaptation or having a better-than-expected outcome (Southwick et al., 2014). Research shows that increased trait resilience is associated with higher levels of subjective well-being and increased optimism and life satisfaction among African Americans college students (Utsey et al., 2008). Other research, viewing resilience as an ongoing process, identified several predictors associated with the process of becoming more resilient (Johnson, 2011). These include (a) participating in learning communities with peers who share oneâs racial or ethnic identity (Leyva, 2021); (b) participating in STEMM pipeline programs (e.g., Lane & Id-Deen, 2020); (c) connecting with social support structures, community, and family (e.g., Fernández, Rincón, & Hinojosa, 2021; Pumaccahua & Rogers, 2022; Rincón, Fernández &. Dueñas, 2020;;); (d) having opportunities to giving back to communities (Page-Reeves et al., 2019; Rincón, & Rodriguez, 2021 (e) affirming oneâs racial identity (e.g., Morton & Parsons 2018; Sparks et al., 2021) including language heritage (Stevenson et al., 2019); (f) engaging in self-renewal activities and religiosity (e.g., meditation) (Gazley & Campbell, 2020); and (g) integrating multiple identities (Ross, Huff, & Godwin, 2021). 5-15 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
There is critical literature examining resilience in STEMM among minoritized individuals (Gonzalez et al., 2021). For example, one recent study explored the factors that allow Black women to be successful and thrive in engineering professions. Interviews with a sample of Black women engineers who had at least 10 years of work experience in industry showed that the integration of race, gender, and role identities fostered resilient engineering identities (Ross et al., 2021). Similarly, longitudinal research shows that Native scholars who have STEMM mentors with knowledge of Native culture are more likely to endorse STEMM values and persist in STEMM (Estrada et al., 2022). Like exiting, grit and resilience are individual-level forms of coping where individuals carve out their own path despite the existence of structural barriers. An overreliance on individualsâ resilience to âmake itâ in STEMM ignores the need for a critical examination of structural barriers in STEMM institutions (McGee & Bentley, 2017; Morton & Nkrumah, 2021) that contribute to the persistence of underrepresentation and exclusion of minoritized individuals, as well as the need to change them. Structural changes in STEMM cultures to promote antiracism, diversity, equity, and inclusion will require more than the advocacy and actions of minoritized individuals with grit or resilience (see Chapter 8 for more). Codeswitching is another way that people from minoritized groups adapt to a predominantly White context and culture temporarily. Codeswitching is a context-dependent impression management strategy where minoritized individuals adjust their self-presentation in predominantly White contexts to fit in and be accepted by mirroring the White majority (McCluney et al., 2021). This occurs through mirroring norms, attributes, and behaviors. Recent codeswitching research shows how minoritized employees may navigate predominately White contexts by dressing differently, changing their speaking style, adopting specific hobbies, and adopting specific topics of conversation (Boulton, 2016; Koch, Gross & Kolts, 2001; McCluney et al., 2021). Through codeswitching, a person acculturates to a predominantly White culture while in the company of White people and switches back to a different, more authentic, self- presentation in non-White environments. For instance, a Black emergency medical doctor from California indicated having to become more familiar with ice hockey and the National Hockey League (NHL) games because that was a popular topic of conversation among his mostly White work colleagues; however, when interacting with another minoritized surgeon, that individual 5-16 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
felt freer to discuss âmaking itâ and facing discrimination in medicine as a person with dreadlocks (Brown, 2021). One form of codeswitching among Black individuals involves changing hairstyles or chemically altering oneâs natural hair to fit into the White norms and rules that define professional appearance in predominately White contexts, including STEMM (Bryant, 2013; Dawson, Karl, & Peluchette, 2019; Donahoo, 2022; Ferguson & Dougherty, 2022; Johnson & Bankhead, 2014; Opie & Phillips, 2015). Codeswitching may be a double-edged sword. One the one hand, there is evidence demonstrating it is associated with increased perceptions of professionalism (McCluney et al., 2021). On the other hand, it reaffirms the expectation that White norms and appearances are the default (McCluney et al., 2019; Ray, 2019; Opie & Phillips, 2015; Rabelo, Robotham, & McCluney, 2021), and it may undermine feelings of authenticity (McCluney et al., 2019). Moreover, minoritized individuals may feel pressured to codeswitch because of psychological penalties for not doing so (Brown, 2021; Dickens & Chavez, 2018; McCluney et al., 2021; Morales et al., 2021). COLLECTIVELY MOBILIZING TO TRANSFORM THE DOMINANT STEMM CULTURE Historically, programs to address the numeric underrepresentation of systemically minoritized groups in STEMM have focused on âfixingâ individualsâe.g., improving scholastic performance, increasing interest, providing research experiences, etc. (Asai, 2020). However, solely relying on this âdeficit-based frameworkâ may lead to the assumption that the cause of the problem is the character of minoritized individuals rather than the persistence of structural barriers (e.g., Coleman & Davis, 2020;; Rocha et al., 2022). It also creates the false impression that minoritized individuals are passive. Shifting to an âasset-basedâ framework (Denton, Borrego, & Boklage, 2020) identifies the strengths, social, and cultural capital among minoritized individuals and returns power to these individuals to counter structural racism; thus, such a shift may prove beneficial (Yosso,2005). Research on validation theory has demonstrated positive effects on student persistence when educators take an asset-based approach by valuing studentsâ strengths, and celebrating inclusion (Barnett, 2011; Linares & Muñoz, 2011). Despite being numerically underrepresented, minoritized students and faculty in STEMM have been active in building welcoming, affinity-based professional communities, both virtual 5-17 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
and in-person, to strengthen their sense of belonging, provide support, expand networks, and grow the next generation in STEMM. For example, the Society for Chicanos, Hispanics, and Native Americans in Science (SACNAS, established in 1973), the National Society for Black Engineers (NSBE, established in 1975), and the American Indian Science and Engineering Society (AISES, established in 1977) are some of the first professional societies to address the paucity of people from minoritized groups in various STEMM disciplines (SACNAS, n.d.; NSBE, n.d.; AISES, 2021). These pioneer organizationsâwhich typically work as membership societies, with dues, student chapters, and annual in-person conferencesâprovide refuge, support, mentorship, professional development, and ingroup role models for emerging scholars (Martin et al., 2016; Ondrechen, 2014; Ross & McGrade, 2016). Qualitative research showed that after attending a SACNAS research conference, students reported increased motivation and networking (Perez & Robnett, 2014). Other research on the impact of SACNAS local chapters showed that Iowa State University students in STEM advanced their careers through regular chapter meetings, workshops, seminars on campus, frequently communicating opportunities from the national SACNAS office, and attending the annual national conference (Flores et al., 2018). Recently, affinity-based communities have sought to use the internet, social media, and social networking platforms to connect minoritized people across geographical distances and surmount the isolation often faced in STEMM. For example, the nonprofit Ciencia Puerto Rico (CienciaPR) was launched in 2006 through a social networking website with the goal of creating a critical mass of scientists from the Puerto Rican community (Guerrero-Medina et al., 2013). In addition to promoting access to information about scientific training pathways and careers, CienciaPR uses its website and social media platforms to counter the negative effects of minoritization in STEMM, challenge the mainstream White-centric narrative by celebrating and affirming Puerto Rican cultural identity in science, and create opportunities for engagement to Puerto Rican members of the STEMM community. Through collaborations with various media outlets, the organization promotes culturally relevant science communication in both Spanish and English that highlights the contributions of Puerto Rican scientists and scientists affiliated with Puerto Rican community and locates scientific advances and information in the context of the lives of Puerto Ricans (Guerrero-Medina et al., 2013). Many of CienciaPRâs programs are designed to enable its community of more than 15,000 scientists to influence their environments 5-18 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
through knowledge and leadership (Colón, 2015; González-Espada et al., 2015). These programs resulted in an engaged community that mobilized following the 2017 Hurricanes Irma and MarÃa, as well as during the COVID-19 pandemic (Forman, 2020; Learning English, 2018; MacArthur et al., 2020; Nirmal, 2021). The number of identity-based organizations and movements has increased in recent years thanks to social media and as a result of large-scale conversations on racism in society (see Box 5-1). Other organizations, such as LatinXinBME, have leveraged new platforms and tools such as Slack and Twitter to overcome geographical divides and isolation in predominantly White contexts. Using Slack has allowed LatinXinBME to create conversation channels centered around specific topics, centralize information relevant to the community, initiate private messages between members, and engage members across time zones. Among the most popular topics are making career connections, mentoring, mental health, and issues of particular concern to Latine communities, such as immigration (Aguado & Porras, 2020). BOX 5-1 Finding Community on Social Media The purpose of this box is to highlight that spaces on social media have been a place where minoritized individuals in STEMM can find community and start mobilizing. As an example, Nature recently published a piece focusing on the emergence of a movement on social media that came about in response to racially motivated incidents (Gewin, 2021). In response to the murder of George Floyd, and the false accusation against the Black birdwatcher Christian Cooper in New York City, the hashtag #BlackBirders was created and ignited the #BlackBirders week. Black researchers and scientists from a range of STEMM disciplines then developed a #BlackinSTEM week series. Each field, for instance physics, received its own week of social media events with the intention of helping diversify and create more inclusion in these environments, generate institutional change, and build community. The Nature article includes the interviews of five researchers who took part in #BlackinSTEM week (Gewin, 2021). [END BOX] Minoritized scientists and health professionals have also recently started mobilizing to examine how the nation teaches STEMM disciplines and to call for the incorporation of 5-19 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
antiracism, diversity, equity, and inclusion into the curriculum. 1 For example, an effort to transform STEMM emerged among Indigenous scholars who noted that there was a disconnect between Western practices of STEMM education and Indigenous ways of knowing and that this dissonance prevented their matriculation into these majors (e.g., Chow-Garcia, 2016; Lee Bitsoi & Lowe, 2018; Smith et al., 2014) (see Chapter 4 for more on Indigenous ways of knowing). Instead of advising students to change and comply with Western practices, Indigenous scholars advocate to create more integrated approaches to STEMM that emphasize the central premise of Indigenous knowledge, which is that all things are connected (Deloria, 1992). Specifically, some scholars have worked to âfind a pedagogy of holism combining the best of bothâ to assist in building capacity (Marker, 2015, p. 3). One group, for example, describe how Indigenous knowledge regarding medicine, technology, math, and anatomy, passing orally across hundreds or even thousands of years can advance STEMM knowledge pursued by the West (Lee Bitsoi & Lowe, 2018). The process of combining involves assessing assumptions, structures, and practices of STEMM. The broadening of STEMM to involve Indigenous students, employees, and local tribal members in the development of these processes is critical to shifting the historical paradigms of these fields, resulting in STEMM fields that broaden to incorporate culturally diverse learners. Furthermore, there have been other recent efforts to transform academic institutions. For instance, minoritized scientists, medical students, and professionals have advocated for change in the form of returning ancestral remains to Indigenous communities (Gulliford, 1996). Others have worked to acknowledge unethical research with Black and Indigenous people. Some individuals have focused on teaching about the history of discrimination against many different peoples in the history of science and medicine in the United States. This has taken many forms, including organizing town halls, teach-ins, op-eds, reading groups, and other initiatives to help STEMM faculty and other members of the scientific community broaden their understanding of the causes and consequences of racism in their respective disciplines (Adelekun et al., 2019; Braun & Saunders, 2017; Hagopian et al., 2018). CONCLUSION 5-1: There are a few noteworthy ways to describe how people from historically and systemically minoritized groups respond to racism in STEMM 1 This section draws on an expert review commissioned by the committee (Smith, 2022). 5-20 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
environments, including other professional spaces. These responses can be loosely grouped as follows: exiting the field, implementing strategies to fit in, and collectively mobilizing to transform the STEMM environment. HOW STEMM PROFESSIONALS CAN HELP In this final section of the chapter, the committee explores specific steps that individuals might take to help promote inclusion through the design of STEMM environments. These steps are grouped into three broad categories: signal inclusion through numeric representation and spatial design of local environments; build peer relationships and community for minoritized groups; and create access to high-status relationships. This section and the recommended actions center Stereotype Inoculation Model, which holds that âsocial vaccinesâ can protect oneâs mind against noxious stereotypes, analogous to biomedical vaccines that protect and inoculate oneâs physical body against noxious bacteria and viruses (Dasgupta, 2011; Stout et al., 2011; Thiem & Dasgupta, 2022). The committeeâs approach is motivated by growing body of research demonstrates that when local STEMM cultures are redesigned to foster social connections among minoritized individuals, increase exposure to experts from minoritized groups, and link STEMM with communal values, these cultural cues act as social vaccines. Specifically, they increase minoritized individualsâ sense of belonging in STEMM, protect against negative stereotypes, and preserve their self-efficacy, motivation, and persistence in STEMM (Dasgupta, 2011; Dennehy & Dasgupta, 2017; Estrada et al., 2021; Thiem & Dasgupta, 2022; Wu, Thiem, & Dasgupta, 2022). This section relies on research on minoritized individualsâ experiences in early training and career stages. However, research on minoritized individuals in middle- and late-stage of their careers is still scarce, and more is needed (see Chapter 9 for the research agenda). SIGNAL INCLUSION THROUGH NUMERIC REPRESENTATION AND SPATIAL DESIGN OF LOCAL ENVIRONMENTS Provide a Diverse Representation of STEMM Role Models It is common for academic organizations and workplaces to elevate some individuals as exemplars of success by raising their portraits on the wall and celebrating their accomplishments 5-21 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
through media stories and awards. These exemplars communicate what âgreatâ looks like within that organization. It is easy to draw the inference that others who look like these success exemplars have potential to be great as well. Conversely, it is easy to infer that people who look different have less greatness potential. Research shows that when young people who are numerically underrepresented in STEMM (e.g., women students in engineering) are exposed to images and success stories of a racially diverse array of successful women engineers, they express more positive outcomes (Wu et al., 2022). These include positive implicit attitudes toward STEMM, reporting more confidence in their own engineering ability, and identifying more strongly with these exemplars, compared to other women students who are exposed to success stories of men engineers or engineering innovations with no mention of the engineerâs gender. In a way, these diverse exemplars of success act as important role models. In fact, women studentsâ subjective identification with success stories of women engineers predicts greater self-confidence in their own ability, which in turn predicts stronger intentions to pursue careers in engineering (Stout et al., 2011). Other research has found that successful individuals are likely to become role models themselves if three key conditions are met. First, there are others that are able to identify with them, second they believe their success is something that can be attained, and third they have overlapping similarities with others (Asgari et al., 2010; Asgari et al., 2012; Aspinwall, 1997; Blanton, 2001; Davies, Spencer, & Steele, 2005; Haines & Kray 2005; Hoyt & Blascovich, 2007; Lockwood, 2006; Lockwood & Kunda, 1997, 1999; Lockwood, Jordan, & Kunda, 2002; Marx & Roman, 2002; Marx, Stapel, & Muller, 2005;;). The dimension of similarity may be similar life history, shared group membership, common academic or professional interests, and so on. For example, research shows that encountering professional women who were framed as similar to the self in terms of background (collegiate or gender) bolsters young womenâs implicit beliefs about their own leadership ability. However, when the professional women were framed as different from the self, there was no effect on womenâs implicit self-beliefs about leadership, and in fact, this even backfired on occasion (Asgari et al., 2012) (for additional reading see Parks-Stamm, Heilman, & Hearns, 2008; Rudman & Phelan, 2010). This latter finding may be explained by the possibility that successful ingroup members who are perceived as different from 5-22 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
oneself may increase feelings of threat. These feelings of threat may signal leadership potential is not possible (Mendes, Blascovich, Major, & Seery, 2001). One longitudinal study found that both quality and quantity of contact with similar exemplars of success jointly enhance young womenâs implicit leadership self-concept and increase ambitious career goals (Asgari et al., 2010). Even though personal contact and mentoring relationships (discussed more below) are powerful ways to find role models, role models may include more indirect contacts, such as with successful individuals with whom one has had only brief contact with or were connected through social media exposure (for a partial review, see Gibson, 2004). Role models are particularly important to minoritized individuals in high-achievement domains (Blanton et al., 2000; Stout et al., 2011). For example, research has demonstrated that after hearing about a high-performing Black peer in a stereotyped field, Black students demonstrated positive outcomes including increased performance and self-efficacy. After hearing about a low-performing Black peer, Black students showed opposite outcomes (Blanton et al., 2000). Moreover, encountering a high-performing Black person is associated with increases in Black studentsâ confidence even more so than what encountering a high-performing White person does for White students. Build a Critical Mass of Minoritized Individuals Building a critical mass of minoritized individuals in a local context reduces feelings of evaluation apprehension and elevates performance and full participation. Research shows that Black womenâs academic performance is enhanced when they are in a group of same-race peers compared to when asked to perform in the context of being the only Black person in an all-White environment (Inzlicht & Ben-Zeev, 2003; Sekaquaptewa & Thompson, 2002). In the presence of other Black peers, negative stereotypes about the group become less prominent in the minds of Black students, feelings of performance apprehension are reduced, and performance is elevated (Sekaquaptewa et al., 2007). Similarly, other research examined womenâs behavior after they were randomly assigned to engineering teams that vary in gender composition (Dasgupta, McManus, & Hunsinger, 2015). Women in engineering teams with a critical mass of same-sex peers expressed less worry and anxiety and were more verbally engaged in the team task compared to women who are the only woman in their team. Even though negative gender 5-23 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
stereotypes were present in their minds, womenâs self-confidence was inoculated against such stereotypes if they were in teams with a critical mass of other women (Dasgupta et al., 2015) (see Chapter 7 for more on teams). Create Congruency Between Expressed Diversity Values and The Reality of the Environment Organizations signal their endorsement of diversity and inclusion values in many ways. One of the most common strategies includes the increased use of diversity and inclusion statements. However, research has found that what minoritized individuals trusted more than diversity and inclusion statements is actually seeing minoritized employees in the workplace (Purdie-Vaughns et al., 2008). For example, pictures showing a diverse group of employees in an organizationâs informational materials increased prospective applicantsâ trust in the company. When the demographic diversity of people was prominent, diversity statements became less important. However, if diversity signals did not match the reality within the company, such measures backfired; new recruits saw the diversity signals as disingenuous and lost trust in the organization (Cheryan et al., 2009). Another feature of local environments that signals diversity values includes the physical design. One study found that the aesthetics of workspaces conveyed clues about its culture, values, and norms (Cheryan et al., 2009). Imagine walking into a workspace where you see vibrant nature posters on the wall and a bookshelf with a variety of books, puzzles, and stacks of water bottles in the corner. Alternatively, imagine that the workspace had Star War posters on the wall, science fiction books in the bookshelf, videogames, and a stack of soda cans in the corner. A room with science fiction paraphernalia and videogames signaled a masculine geeky culture, while a room with nature posters and puzzles signaled that all genders are welcome. Women felt more welcome and, in turn, expressed more interest in computer science classes and majors if they were in the room with nature posters than the one with Star Wars posters. Additional research should be conducted to examine which specific features of a physical environment are most inclusive to promoting ADEI in STEMM. 5-24 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
BUILD PEER RELATIONSHIPS AND COMMUNITY FOR MINORITIZED GROUPS Broker Ingroup Peer Relationships by Developing âNear Peerâ Mentors Peer mentorship programs that broker relationships between new students from minoritized groups with other students who share their identity is another feature of local environments that enhance belonging and thriving (Dennehy & Dasgupta, 2017; Herrmann et al., 2016). These ânear peers,â who are only slightly more senior, mentor early career colleagues, a form of mentorship that has been found to be effective, including for minoritized individuals in STEMM environments (NASEM 2020; Dennehy & Dasgupta, 2017; Rockinson-Szapkiw & Wendt, 2020; Thiem & Dasgupta, 2022; Wu, Thiem, & Dasgupta, 2022). Authentic relationships with successful ingroup peers safeguard individuals from the impacts of negative ingroup stereotypes. For example, a one-year mentoring relationship with a same-sex peer mentor during the first year of college was enormously effective for female engineering students at protecting them against academic anxiety. Same-sex peer mentoring also protected the female studentsâ well-being, enabled them to have increased success in obtaining experiential learning opportunities (e.g., engineering internships), and increased their retention in engineering and other STEM majors (Dennehy & Dasgupta, 2017; Wu, Thiem, & Dasgupta, 2022). These benefits achieved by having same-sex mentors were not only present in the first year of college when mentoring was active, but also endured through graduation and one-year post-graduation (Dennehy & Dasgupta, 2017; Wu et al., 2022). Having male peer mentors in the first year of college produced results statistically no different from having no mentors (Dennehy & Dasgupta, 2017; Wu et al., 2022). In short, a low-cost, light-touch near peer mentorship experience in a critical transition period in life yielded dividends through the college years and one year after graduation, long after mentorship has ended. One benefit of ingroup peer mentors and other peer role models derives from their relatability and shared experiences. For example, a national sample of African American undergraduates majoring in STEM disciplines found that from these studentsâ perspectives, perceived similarity of values rather than demographic similarity was the most important factor associated with protégé perceptions of high-quality mentorship, which in turn was associated with stronger personal commitment to pursue STEM careers (Hernandez et al., 2017). 5-25 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
Normalize Struggles and Strengths of Minoritized Groups Hearing that adversity and struggle is a normal experience and part of the journey bolsters persistence and performance. For example, Black students who learned from a slightly more senior peer that all students encounter challenges and that the challenges are usually temporary experienced positive outcomes. Specifically, they were not concerned about whether they belonged in college, but instead attributed their challenges to the broader college environment. Consequently, these students with the senior peer were more likely than Black students in a control condition to achieve higher grades and improved well-being (Walton & Cohen, 2011). As a corollary, emphasizing the strengths that come from the culture, history, and knowledge of historically and systemically minoritized groups can also empower numerically underrepresented students in higher education, such as first-generation working-class students (Stephens et al., 2014; Townsend et al., 2019). For example, first-generation students learning from senior first-generation peers about the positive and negative effects of their working-class status on their overall college experience reduced both anxiety and bolstered college (Stephens et al., 2014). Research with 670 STEM minority undergraduates (Black and African American and Latine) and non-minority (White and Asian students) found that an intervention that raised studentsâ awareness about stereotype threat and encouraged them to use their lived experiences to generate coping strategies that work for them protected their abstract reasoning, increased course grades, and decreased worries about confirming racial and ethnic stereotypes (Ben-Zeev, et. al., 2017). Create Affinity-Based Peer Programs When young people transition from high school to college, they are often in new environments and unmoored from prior social supports, and so, often experience uncertainty about belonging and self-doubt. This is especially likely for students from minoritized groups who are also numerically underrepresented in higher education. Affinity-based programs that gather a cohort of similarly situated students in the transition to college have been found to be very effective. For example, summer bridge programs that take place during the summer before college starts help first-generation students, low-income students, and students from minoritized racial 5-26 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
and ethnic groups make a smoother transition to college (Ramirez et al., 2020; Strayhorn, 2011). Such programs enhance college-related social capital through an immersive on-campus experience, spanning several weeks, that strengthens socioemotional ties among peers, builds relationships with faculty and staff, and familiarizes students with campus resources (Ashley et al., 2017; Bradford et al., 2021; Suzuki et al., 2012). A systematic review of STEM-specific summer bridge programs found that students leave these programs with increased feelings of preparedness and belonging more in college, and they get better grades post-program (Ashley et al., 2017). A recent meta-analysis examining STEM-specific summer bridge programs found similar positive outcomes (Bradford et al., 2021). Summer bridge program participants, including minoritized, and first-generation students are also more likely to graduate within six years of college matriculation than a matched comparison group (Douglas & Attewell, 2014). Another study on summer bridge programs showed that including a component that has new students from minoritized groups hearing from senior peers from similar identity groups plays a key role in their success. One study, for example, looked at outcomes from a sample of summer bridge program participants in which most participants were from minoritized racial and ethnic groups, first-generation students, and/or low-income students (Ramirez, 2020). All participants received information about academic resources available to them on campus, but only half of the participants also heard from senior first-generation college students. These senior students told stories about encountering and overcoming identity related obstacles. Program participants who heard from these senior peers reported greater interest in using campus resources compared to program participants who did not hear from them. Given the demonstrated benefits of summer bridge programs, additional research is needed to identify which specific elements of these programs drive each outcome. Living-learning student communities are another affinity-based peer program. These communities foster socioemotional and academic relationships among a cohort of students transitioning into college who have common academic interests or a shared identity. Students live together in the same residence hall, attend at least one class together, and interact together in structured activities (Brower & Inkelas, 2010). Consistent with the Stereotype Inoculation Model, immersion in a community of ingroup peers increases psychological fit within the local environment. This is especially important for students who are in a small numeric minority in the context of their larger university environment. The local community, in this case, is a space in 5-27 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
which students are protected from tokenization and doubts about their belonging, bolstering their confidence and persistence (Thiem & Dasgupta, 2022). Research shows that as compared to those who are not participants, those in living- learning communities have more frequent academic interactions with their peers, and they experience a stronger sense of belonging within their university and living-learning community (Dahl et al., 2020; Schussler & Fierros, 2008; Wawrzynski et al., 2009; Wu et al., accepted in principle). Studies demonstrate that the social support from the community peers also increases academic persistence (Soldner et al., 2012; Inkelas et al., 2007) and reduces academic anxiety. These factors have been found to help students earn higher grades (Wu et al., accepted in principle). As with summer bridge programs, research suggests that living-learning communities are particularly beneficial for orienting students to campus resources. Compared to students living in traditional residential campus communities, students participating in living-learning communities are more likely to use campus resources such as computer labs, academic advisors, and peer counselors, and they are more invested in participating in research, studying abroad, and conducting a thesis project (Brower & Inkelas, 2010; Dahl et al., 2020; Inkelas et al., 2007). This pattern of findings may occur as a result of increased knowledge of these opportunities and increased confidence they may experience. They are also more likely to maintain higher academic confidence or self-efficacy up to three years later after a program is over (Brower & Inkelas, 2010). Importantly, research has identified value of living-learning communities for women of color in STEM (Johnson, 2011). In addition, communal aspects of college are also emphasized through living-learning communities. These communities show that learning is not something that only occurs in classrooms. Rather, learning can happen in informal spaces with peers, through activities, and within residence halls outside of class (Brower & Inklas, 2010; Shapiro & Levine, 1999). Furthermore, these communities may help students connect with faculty through informal low stakes one-on-one interactions (Eidum et al., 2020). Programs that create more opportunities for faculty-student interactions are likely to attract faculty members who are more communally oriented, and research shows that students who prioritize communal values may have a preference to interact with faculty who share similar values (Fuesting & Diekman, 2017). 5-28 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
Therefore, minoritized students, who may be more likely to prioritize communal values, may appreciate this particular aspect of living-learning communities. Curriculum Change Initiatives Curriculum change initiatives also contribute to increased performance and persistence for minoritized students. One significant intervention to increase inclusion has occurred with the adoption of course-based undergraduate research experiences, sometimes called Freshman Research Initiatives). During the research experiences, faculty provide students with opportunities to engage in discovery as part of a course, as opposed to a more traditional instructional model. Students work in collaborative teams and take part in each element of the research process, including developing research questions, collecting data, analyzing data, and drawing conclusions (Alkaher & Dolan, 2014; Auchincloss et al, 2014; Bangera & Brownell, 2014; Weaver et al, 2008). Studies on these research experiences show that this shift in the curriculum towards doing âauthenticâ research results in a range of positive outcomes that include greater knowledge acquisition, increased self-efficacy and shifts in psychosocial outcomes, a greater sense of belonging in the scientific community, increased retention, increased persistence for all students, and an increased science identity (Alkaher & Dolan, 2014; Brownell et al, 2013; Drew & Triplett, 2008; Rodenbusch et al, 2016; Hanauer et al., 2012; Jordan et al., 2014; Lopatto et al., 2008; Shaffer et al., 2010; Shaffer et al., 2014). Increases in science efficacy, identity, and values have been shown to predict persistence for minoritized STEM students one year and four years after graduation (Estrada et al., 2011; Estrada, Hernandez, & Schultz, 2018). Moreover, these courses equalize retention rates for minoritized students compared to non-minoritized students (Rodenbusch et al., 2016), which is significant, given that first-year courses contribute towards large numbers of minoritized students with an expressed interest in STEMM leaving their STEMM career pathways. Despite critiques (Linn et al., 2015), several national reports have recommended course-based undergraduate research experiences as an important mechanism for increasing persistence and retention of students (National Academy of Sciences, 2016; PCAST, 2012). The measured psychosocial shifts indicate that in addition to affecting learning outcomes, course curriculum changes potentially provide increased affiliation cues and affirm student dignity and inclusion for minoritized students. 5-29 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
CREATE ACCESS TO HIGH-STATUS RELATIONSHIPS Because of long-standing barriers, students from minoritized groups may be unable to cultivate meaningful relationships with people in higher-status roles, such as managers, professors, and other organizational leaders. These relationships are critical for opening doors to important opportunities for students, such as internship positions, career advice, and future letters of recommendation (e.g., Castilla et al., 2013a; Castilla et al., 2013b). For example, many working-class students who are the first in their families to attend college and do not have parents helping them navigate college life may not recognize the importance of forming these relationships or may not know how to cultivate them (Collier & Morgan, 2008; Jack, 2016). Even students who do understand the importance of faculty relationships may feel uncomfortable and self-conscious approaching high-status people they do not know out of a fear that doing so will make them be perceived as a âsuck-upâ (Collier & Morgan, 2008; Jack, 2016). Consequently, students, including minoritized students, often avoid approaching faculty members. Enable Access to High-Status Mentors Mentorship plays an essential role in the development and career trajectories of emerging STEMM professionals. Much of science professional development is based on an apprenticeship or mentorship model in which junior trainees work directly with other STEMM professionals and develop through their guidance. Mentorship is âa reciprocal, dynamic relationship between mentor (or mentoring team) and mentee that promotes the satisfaction and development of bothâ (McGee, 2016; p. 232). Mentoring relationships are complex and have a substantial impact on the professional and personal lives of the mentees (NASEM, 2020). Mentees are not passive recipients of their mentorsâ guidance. Ideally, they and their mentors collaborate in reciprocal ways as they plan, act, reflect, question, and problem-solve (Pfund et al., 2016). While mentees acquire research skills they need to be scientifically productive and build professional knowledge to advance their careers, mentors acquire skills that enable them to nurture the academic and professional growth of the next generation more effectively. Mentors are successful if they can support their mentee in attaining transferable skills, knowledge, and confidence necessary to meet the latterâs individual goals. This requires mentors to understand their menteesâ unique needs and desires, as well as the flexibility and 5-30 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
humility to adjust their approach to support the menteesâ success. In addition to disciplinary training, successful mentees also must acquire the tacit âknow how,â or social capital, to navigate political and disciplinary minefields, maintain personal and professional integrity, and learn the values of their professional fields (Csikszentmihalyi, 2009; Yosso, 2005; Zambrana et al., 2015). The positive impacts of mentoring on student mentees are clear. Mentors contribute to studentsâ science identity by recognizing their menteesâ talent, teaching them what to do, validating their aspirations, and providing opportunities for mentees to take on more responsibility (Chemers et al., 2011; Hurtado et al., 2009). More generally, strong mentorship is associated with menteesâ self-efficacy (Laursen et al., 2010). Mentorship also benefits the mentor by fostering a sense of fulfillment, sharpening mentorsâ leadership skills, and increasing their self-awareness (Dolan & Johnson, 2009; Laursen et al., 2010). Increased productivity among research mentees is associated with increased productivity for research mentors (Dolan & Johnson, 2009). For minoritized students in STEM, mentorship enhances pursuit of research- related career pathways (Nagda et al., 1998; Hathaway, Nagda, Gregerman, 2002). One study examined factors that predicted the integration of minoritized individuals in the STEM environment from studentsâ junior year through the postbaccalaureate year (Estrada, Hernandez, & Schultz, 2018). Researchers assessed menteesâ perceptions of the extent to which their mentor provided quality psychosocial, networking, and instrumental support. Results showed longitudinal support demonstrating that quality mentorship experiences were significantly associated with the integration of minoritized individuals in STEM (Estrada et al., 2018). Despite these promising findings, research demonstrates that Black, Latine, and Indigenous individuals typically receive less mentoring than their non-minoritized peers (Thomas, 2001; Morzinski & Fisher, 2002; Ginther et al., 2011). Mentoring relationships are influenced by the mentor and menteeâs perceptions of the other (Byars-Winston et al., 2020) and are culturally informed in ways that vary by race and ethnicity (Byars-Winston et al., 2015; Byars-Winston et al., 2020; Prunuske et al., 2013). For example, research has shown that minoritized women in STEM are more interested in having discussions pertaining to issues of race and ethnicity with their mentors, as compared to their 5-31 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
White counterparts (Muller et al., 2012). 2 Differences in priorities between mentors and mentees regarding race and ethnicity can create a disconnect, and can potentially hurt minoritized mentees (Byars-Winston et al., 2015; Byars-Winston et al., 2020). One qualitative study examined perspectives on mentoring minoritized students that were held by a sample of White faculty who did not often mentor minoritized individuals (McCoy et al., 2015). In the interviews, their use of race-neutral, âcolorblindâ language (avoiding racial terms but implying them) allowed these White faculty members to describe their students as inferior, both academically and inferior across a range of perceptions, while potentially ignoring issues related to structural racism. The faculty stated they utilized colorblind methods to âtreat everyone equal;â however, the findings showed that their colorblind approach had negative implications for their perception of the mentees, including the assertion of their mentees as being inadequately prepared. Misalignment of diversity related beliefs and expectations between mentors and mentees can complicate mentoring relationships (Carlone & Johnson, 2007; Chang et al., 2011; Hurtado et al., 2009; Johnson et al., 2011). Some programs, such as the NIH-funded National Research Mentorship Network (NRMN), aims to strengthen alignment between mentees and mentors by teaching faculty five clusters of skills (McGee; 2016; Pfund et al., 2016). These include: (1) teaching and evaluating research skills, (2) building honest and trusting mentor-mentee relationships; (3) providing psychosocial support (motivation, coping strategies, belonging); (4) being culturally responsive (being self-aware of bias, reducing impacts of stereotype threat); and (5) being an active sponsor (helping mentees network, advocating for them, promoting their professional development). Students from minoritized groups may be most comfortable forming working relationships with faculty of similar identities (Blake-Beard et al., 2011). However, as demonstrated in earlier chapters (see Chapter 3), few faculty are members of minoritized groups. This limits the chances that minoritized students have to interact with them. As an added barrier, the few minoritized faculty members tend to be over-extended. In part, this results from the fact that they are highly sought out by students with similar identities (Hirshfield & Joseph, 2012; Mondisa, 2018; Padilla, 1994; Stanley, 2006 Zambrana et al., 2015). Which is to say, numeric 2 Differences in priorities between mentors and mentees regarding race and ethnicity can create a disconnect, and can potentially hurt minoritized mentees (Byars-Winston et al., 2015; Byars-Winston et al., 2020). 5-32 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
underrepresentation of minoritized faculty can contribute to the limited professional networks of students from minoritized groups. It is important, therefore, for academic STEMM departments to form relationships with STEMM organizations dedicated to nurturing diverse talent, such as SACNAS, NSBE, and the Annual Biomedical Conference for Minoritized Scientists. In addition, it is important for STEMM departments to create and support local chapters of these organizations and create annual opportunities for cohorts of students to travel to their conferences to build a network of peers and professionals from similar identity groups. These in- person connections can be supplemented with participation in virtual groups for minoritized scholars in STEMM (e.g., @LatinXinBME). Additional research is needed to examine the potential impact of other high-status individuals, including sponsors and champions, in other STEMM organizational environments beyond higher education. ORGANIZATION OF THE RECOMMENDATIONS This concluding section offers recommendations aligned with the core areas of empirical research discussed in the chapter. Specifically, Recommendation 5-1 corresponds to signaling inclusion through numeric representation and spatial design of local environments. Recommendation 5-2 corresponds to building peer relationships and community for minoritized groups. Recommendation 5-3 corresponds to creating access to high-status relationships. Recommendation 5-4 corresponds to additional strategies that are centered on fostering connections, emphasizing communal values, affirming kindness cues and increasing an overall sense of belonging. The final, Recommendation 5-5, is centered on continuing research experience curriculum-based initiatives. RECOMMENDATION 5-1: Leaders and gatekeepers of STEMM organizations, higher education, and human resource offices can improve minoritized peopleâs individual and interpersonal experiences in STEMM educational and professional environments through the following practices: 1. Improve numerical diversity through the admission, hire, and inclusion of minoritized individuals at all levels of an organization: a. Establish information systems across institutions using common metrics for comparison purposes to collect data, track success, and 5-33 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
identify areas of numeric disparities. Results should be transparent, up-to-date, and accurate. b. Hire more minoritized individuals, especially in positions where minoritized role models are often missing (e.g., leadership, mentorship), with the aim of building a critical mass. c. Determine if the institutional diversity statement reflects the reality of the institutional environment, and directly address discrepancies. d. Adapt curriculum, physical environment, media stories, and other content to incorporate more examples of minoritized role models. RECOMMENDATION 5-2: Leaders and gatekeepers of STEMM organizations, higher education, and human resource offices can improve minoritized peopleâs individual and interpersonal experiences in STEMM educational and professional environments through the following practices: 1. Create and provide continued investment in evidence-based programs that connect minoritized individuals to ingroup peers, institutional resources, and professional networks. These investments require significant expertise in their designs and execution, and they may not yield immediate results; however, they can increase a sense of welcome and belonging through the ability to connect with individuals from similar racial and ethnic backgrounds. Types of programs may include the following types of resources: a. Summer bridge programs. b. Living-learning communities. c. Peer and near-peer mentorship programs. d. Actively work to form relationships with national-level affinity societies (e.g., SACNAS, NSBE, AISES, etc.), create local chapters, and provide opportunities for minoritized individuals to connect with them. RECOMMENDATION 5-3: Leaders and gatekeepers of STEMM organizations, higher education, and human resource offices can improve minoritized peopleâs 5-34 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
individual and interpersonal experiences in STEMM educational and professional environments through the following practices: 1. Create and provide continued investment in programs that facilitate working relationships between minoritized individuals to high-status professionals: a. Create and invest in mentorship programs, while also hiring more minoritized faculty. b. Conduct additional research examining the roles of other high-status individuals such as champions and sponsors on fostering STEMM careers for minoritized individuals. RECOMMENDATION 5-4: Leaders and gatekeepers of STEMM organizations, higher education, and human resource offices can improve minoritized peopleâs individual and interpersonal experiences in STEMM educational and professional environments through the following practices: 1. Develop interpersonal environments and institutional norms that promote inclusion, dignity, belonging and affirmations of kindness: a. Actively recognize minoritized individualsâ contributions to STEMM across multiple mediums such as portraits, media stories, awards, etc. b. De-center White professional norms in culture, dress, and appearance. c. Conduct additional research examining which features of the physical environment are most likely to promote sustainable ADEI in STEMM. d. Emphasize and recognize the importance of communal values in STEMM work. e. Redesign STEMM curriculum to incorporate Indigenous ways of knowing, and actively involve Indigenous communities in the development of this process. f. Create cultural norms that communicate the strengths and struggles of minoritized groups. 5-35 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
g. Provide access to culturally responsive mental health providers or resources with experience in addressing racial stress, trauma, and aggressions for minoritized individuals who have experienced distress and would like to pursue these options. h. Conduct bi-annual âcultural auditsâ to determine if the institution is fostering an environment of inclusion. RECOMMENDATION 5-5: Leaders and gatekeepers of STEMM organizations, higher education, and human resource offices can improve minoritized peopleâs individual and interpersonal experiences in STEMM educational and professional environments through the following practices: 1. Use evidence-based design and implementation practices to build curriculum initiatives that increase access to discovery, including, for example, course- based research experiences (CRE). 5-36 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
REFERENCES Adelekun, A. A., Beltrán, S., Carney, J., Lett, E., Orji, W. U., Rider-Longmaid, E., ... & Aysola, J. (2019). Recognizing racism in medicine: a student-organized and community-engaged health professional conference. Health Equity, 3(1), 395-402. Aguado, B. A., & Porras, A. M. (2020). Building a virtual community to support and celebrate the success of Latinx scientists. Nature Reviews Materials, 5(12), 862-864. American Indian Science and Engineering Society. (2021, March 23). Our history. AISES. Retrieved January 12, 2023, from https://www.aises.org/about/history Albuja, A. F., Sanchez, D. T., & Gaither, S. E. (2019). Identity denied: Comparing American or White identity denial and psychological health outcomes among bicultural and biracial people. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 45(3), 416â43 Alkaher, I., & Dolan, E. L. (2014). Integrting research into undergraduate courses: Current practices and future directions. In D. Sunal, C. Sunal, D. Zollman, C. Mason, & E. Wright (Eds.), Research in Science Education: Research Based Undergraduate Science Teaching (pp. 403-434). Charlotte, NC: Information Age. American Council on Education (ACE) (2005). Increasing the Success of Minority Students in Science and Technology. Washington, DC. https://www.acenet.edu/Documents/Increasing-the-Success- of-Minority-Students-in-Science-and-Technology-2006.pdf Araújo, B. Y., & Borrell, L. N. (2006). Understanding the link between discrimination, mental health outcomes, and life chances among Latinos. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 28(2), 245-266. Aronson J, Inzlicht M. The Ups and Downs of Attributional Ambiguity: Stereotype Vulnerability and the Academic Self-Knowledge of African American College Students. Psychological Science. 2004;15(12):829-836. Aronson, J., & Steele, C.M. (2005). Stereotypes and the fragility of human competence, motivation, and self-concept. In C. Dweck & E. Elliot (Eds.), Handbook of competence & motivation. New York, NY: Guilford. Asai, D. J. (2020). Race matters. Cell, 181(4), 754-757. Asgari, S., Dasgupta, N., & Cote, N. G. (2010). When does contact with successful ingroup members change self-stereotypes? A longitudinal study comparing the effect of quantity vs. quality of contact with successful individuals. Social Psychology, 41(3), 203. Asgari, S., Dasgupta, N., & Stout, J. G. (2012). When do counterstereotypic ingroup members inspire versus deflate? The effect of successful professional women on young womenâs leadership self- concept. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(3), 370-383. Ashley, M., Cooper, K. M., Cala, J. M., & Brownell, S. E. (2017). Building better bridges into STEM: A synthesis of 25 years of literature on STEM summer bridge programs. CBEâLife Sciences Education, 16(4), es3. Aspinwall, L. G. (1997). Future-oriented aspects of social comparisons: A framework for studying health- related comparison activity. Health, coping, and well-being: Perspectives from social comparison theory, 125-165. Aspinwall, L. G. (2004). Dealing with Adversity: Self-regulation, Coping, Adaptation, and Health. In M. B. Brewer & M. Hewstone (Eds.), Applied social psychology (pp. 3â27). Blackwell Publishing. Auchincloss, L. C., Laursen, S. L., Branchaw, J. L., Eagan, K., Graham, M., Hanauer, D. I., ... & Dolan, E. L. (2014). Assessment of course-based undergraduate research experiences: a meeting report. Bandyopadhyay, S., Boylan, C. T., Baho, Y.G., Casey, A., Asif, A., Khalil, H., Badwi, N., & Patel, R. (2022) Ethnicity-related stereotypes and their impacts on medical students: A critical narrative review of health professions education literature, Medical Teacher, Bangera, G., & Brownell, S. E. (2014). Course-based undergraduate research experiences can make scientific research more inclusive. CBE Life Sci Educ, 13(4), 602-606. doi:10.1187/cbe.14-06-0099 5-37 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
Barnett, E. A. (2011). Validation experiences and persistence among community college students. The Review of Higher Education, 34(2), 193-230. Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological bulletin, 117(3), 497-529. Ben-Zeev, A., Paluy, Y., Milless, K. L., Wallace, L., Marquez-Magana, L., Bibbins-Domingo, K., & Estrada, M. (2017). âSpeaking truthâ protects underrepresented minoritiesâ intellectual performance and safety in STEM. Education sciences, 7(2), 65. Black, L. L., Johnson, R., & VanHoose, L. (2015). The relationship between perceived racism/discrimination and health among black American women: a review of the literature from 2003 to 2013. Journal of racial and ethnic health disparities, 2(1), 11-20. Blair, I. V., Judd, C. M., & Chapleau, K. M. (2004). The influence of Afrocentric facial features in criminal sentencing. Psychological science, 15(10), 674â679. BlakeâBeard, S., Bayne, M. L., Crosby, F. J., & Muller, C. B. (2011). Matching by race and gender in mentoring relationships: Keeping our eyes on the prize. Journal of Social issues, 67(3), 622-643. Blake-Beard, S., Bayne, M.L., Crosby, F.J. & Muller, C.B. (2011) Matching by race and gender in mentoring relationships: Keeping our eyes on the prize. Journal of Social Issues, 67(3), 622â643. Blanton, H. (2013). Evaluating the self in the context of another: The three-selves model of social comparison assimilation and contrast. In Cognitive social psychology (pp. 79-91). Psychology Press. Blanton, H., Crocker, J., & Miller, D. T. (2000). The effects of in-group versus out-group social comparison on self-esteem in the context of a negative stereotype. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36(5), 519-530. Blascovich J, Spencer SJ, Quinn D, Steele C. (2001) African Americans and High Blood Pressure: The Role of Stereotype Threat. Psychological Science,12(3):225-229. Böhm, R., Rusch, H., & Baron, J. (2020). The psychology of intergroup conflict: A review of theories and measures. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 178, 947-962. Boulton, C. (2016). Black identities inside advertising: Race inequality, code switching, and stereotype threat. Howard Journal of Communications, 27(2), 130-144. Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Loss. (Vol. 1): Basic Books. Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation (Vol. 1): Basic Books. Bradford, B. C., Beier, M. E., & Oswald, F. L. (2021). A meta-analysis of university stem summer bridge program effectiveness. CBEâLife Sciences Education, 20(2), ar21. Braun, L., & Saunders, B. (2017). Avoiding racial essentialism in medical science curricula. AMA Journal of Ethics, 19(6), 518-527. Bravata, D.M., Watts, S.A., Keefer, A.L., Madhusudhan, D.K., Taylor, K.T., Clark, D.M., Nelson, R.S., Cokley, K.O., Hagg, H.K.. Prevalence, Predictors, and Treatment of Impostor Syndrome: a Systematic Review. J Gen Intern Med. 2020 Apr;35(4):1252-1275. doi: 10.1007/s11606-019-05364- 1. Epub 2019 Dec 17. PMID: 31848865; PMCID: PMC7174434. Brewer, M. B. (2007). The social psychology of intergroup relations: Social categorization, ingroup bias, and outgroup prejudice. In A. W. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 695â715). The Guilford Press. Brower, A. M., & Inkelas, K. K. (2010). Living-learning programs: One high-impact educational practice we now know a lot about. Liberal Education, 96(2), 36-43. Brown, I. M. (2021). Diversity matters: code-switching as an EM survival tactic. Emergency Medicine News, 43(1), 7. Brown, R. (1986). Social psychology. Simon and Schuster. Brown, R. (2000). Social identity theory: past achievements, current problems and future challenges. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 634-667. Brownell, S. E., Kloser, M. J., Fukami, T., & Shavelson, R. J. (2013). Context matters: volunteer bias, small sample size, and the value of comparison groups in the assessment of research-based undergraduate introductory biology lab courses. J Microbiol Biol Educ, 14, 176-182. 5-38 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
Bryant, S. L. (2013). The beauty ideal: the effects of European standards of beauty on Black women. Columbia Social Work Review, 11(1), 80-91 Bryant-Davis T, Ocampo C. Racist IncidentâBased Trauma. The Counseling Psychologist. 2005;33(4):479-500. Byars-Winston A, Branchaw J, Pfund C, Leverett P, Newton J. (2015). Culturally diverse undergraduate researchersâ academic outcomes and perceptions of their research mentoring relationships. International Journal of Science Education. 37(15):2533â54. Byars-Winston, A., Leverett, P., Benbow, R. J., Pfund, C., Thayer-Hart, N., & Branchaw, J. (2020). Race and Ethnicity in Biology Research Mentoring Relationships. Journal of Diversity in Higher Education, 13(3), 240â253. https://doi.org/10.1037/dhe0000106 Callagher, L. J., El Sahn, Z., Hibbert, P., Korber, S., & Siedlok, F. (2021). Early career researchersâ identity threats in the field: The shelter and shadow of collective support. Management Learning, 52(4), 442â465. https://doi.org/10.1177/1350507621997738 Carlone, H. B., & Johnson, A. (2007). Understanding the science experiences of successful women of color: Science identity as an analytic lens. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 44(8), 1187- 1218. Casella, E., & Fowler, C. (2005). The archaeology of plural and changing identities: beyond identification: Springer. Castilla, E.J. (2008). Gender, race, and meritocracy in organizational careers. American Journal of Sociology,113(6), Castilla, E.J., Lan, G.J. & Rissing, B.A. (2013a) Social networks and employment: Mechanisms(Part 1). Sociology Compass, 7(12), 999â1012. Castilla, E.J., Lan, G.J. & Rissing, B.A. (2013b) Social networks and employment: Outcomes (Part 2). Sociology Compass, 7(12), 1013â1026. 507â516 Ceci, S. J., & Williams, W.M. (2010). The mathematics of sex: How biology and society conspire to limit talented women and girls. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Ceci, S. J., Williams, W. M., & Barnett, S. M. (2009). Womenâs underrepresentation in science: Sociocultural and biological considerations. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 281â261. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) (2021). Retrieved from https://www.cdc.gov/media/releases/2021/s0408-racism-health.html Chae, D. H., Lincoln, K. D., & Jackson, J. S. (2011). Discrimination, attribution, and racial group identification: Implications for psychological distress among Black Americans in the National Survey of American Life (2001â2003). American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 81(4), 498â506. Chae, J. H., Piedmont, R. L., Estadt, B. K., & Wicks, R. J. (1995). Personological evaluation of Clanceâs Imposter Phenomenon Scale in a Korean sample. Journal of Personality Assessment, 65, 468â485. Chang, M. J., Eagan, M. K., Lin, M. H., & Hurtado, S. (2011). Considering the impact of racial stigmas and science identity: Persistence among biomedical and behavioral science aspirants. The Journal of Higher Education, 82(5), 564-596. Chemers, M. M., Zurbriggen, E. L., Syed, M., Goza, B. K., & Bearman, S. (2011). The role of efficacy and identity in science career commitment among underrepresented minority students. Journal of Social Issues, 67(3), 469-491. Cheney, D., Seyfarth, R., & Smuts, B. (1986). Social relationships and social cognition in nonhuman primates. Science, 234, 1361-1367. Cheryan, S., Plaut, V. C., Davies, P. G., & Steele, C. M. (2009). Ambient belonging: how stereotypical cues impact gender participation in computer science. Journal of personality and social psychology, 97(6), 1045. Chow-Garcia, N. (2016). Educational Pathways for Native American Graduates: Stories through Natives in STEM 14 the STEM Bachelorâs, Masterâs, and Doctoral Degrees (Doctoral dissertation, University of Rochester. 5-39 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
Chrisman, S.M., Pieper, W.A., Clance, P.R., Holland, C.L., & Glickauf-Hughes, C. (1995). Validation of the clance imposter phenomenon scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 65, 456â467. Christopoulou, M., Lakioti, A., Pezirkianidis, C., Karakasidou, E., & Stalikas, A. (2018). The role of grit in education: A systematic review. Psychology, 9(15), 2951-2971. Clance, P. R. (1985). The imposter phenomenon: Overcoming the fear that haunts your success. Atlanta, GA: Peachtree. Clance, P. R., & Imes, S. A. (1978). The imposter phenomenon in high achieving women: Dynamics and therapeutic intervention. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, and Practice, 15, 241â247. Coleman, A. (2020). D-STEM Equity Model: Diversifying the STEM education to career pathway. https://www.athensjournals.gr/education/2020-7-3-3-Coleman.pdf Collier, P. J., & Morgan, D. L. (2008). âIs that paper really due today?â: Differences in first-generation and traditional college studentsâ understandings of faculty expectations. Higher education, 55(4), 425-446. Colón, J. (2015). Puerto Rico's future at stake. Science, 349(6253), 1145-1145. Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., and Barkow, J. H. (1992). Introduction: Evolutionary psychology and conceptual integration. In Jerome Barkow, Leda Cosmides & John Tooby (Eds.), The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture (pp. 3-15). England, UK: Oxford University Press. Cozzarelli, C., & Major, B. (1990). Exploring the validity of the impostor phenomenon. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 9, 401â417. Crocker, J., Karpinski, A., Quinn, D. M., & Chase, S. K. (2003). When grades determine self-worth: Consequences of contingent self-worth for male and female engineering and psychology majors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, Crocker, J., Major, B., & Steele, C. M. (1998). Social stigma. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., pp. 504â553). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. Crocker, J., Voelkl, K., Testa, M., & Major, B. (1991). Social stigma: The affective consequences of attributional ambiguity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(2), 218â228. Csikszentmihalyi M. (2009) Forward. In: J. Nakamura, Shernoff D., Hooker C. (Eds) . Good mentoring: Fostering excellent practice in higher education. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Dahl, L. S., Duran, A., Hooten, Z. J., Stipeck, C. J., & Youngerman, E. (2020). Investigating the Influence of Residential Learning Communities on Student Experiences. Learning Communities: Research & Practice, 8(1), 6. Danesh, J., et al. (2000). Low grade inflammation and coronary heart disease: prospective study and updated meta-analyses. BMJ, 321(7255), 199e204. Social Science & Medicine, 77, 75-83. Das, A. (2013). How does race get âunder the skinâ?: Inflammation, weathering, and metabolic problems in late life. Social Science & Medicine, 77, 75-83. How does race get âunder the skinâ?: Inflammation, weathering, and metabolic problems in late life - ScienceDirect Dasgupta, N. (2011). Ingroup experts and peers as social vaccines who inoculate the self-concept: The Stereotype Inoculation Model. Psychological Inquiry, 22, 231-246. Dasgupta, N., Scircle, M. M., & Hunsinger, M. (2015). Female peers in small work groups enhance women's motivation, verbal participation, and career aspirations in engineering. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(16), 4988-4993. Davies, P. G., Spencer, S. J., & Steele, C. M. (2005). Clearing the air: identity safety moderates the effects of stereotype threat on women's leadership aspirations. Journal of personality and social psychology, 88(2), 276. Dawson, G. A., Karl, K. A., & Peluchette, J. V. (2019). Hair matters: Toward understanding natural black hair bias in the workplace. Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies, 26(3), 389-401. De Waal, F. (1990). Peacemaking among primates: Harvard University Press. De Waal, F. (2009). The age of empathy: Harmony. Debb, S. M., Colson, D., Hacker, D., & Park, K. (2018). Applying the Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale for use with third-year African American college students. Journal of Negro Education, 87(1), 73-89. 5-40 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
Deloria, V., Jr. (1992). Relativity, relatedness and reality. Winds of Change, 7(4), 34-40. Dennehy, T.C & Dasgupta, N. (2017). Female peer mentors early in college increase womenâs positive academic experiences and retention in engineering. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(23), 5964-5969. Dennehy, T.C., Smith, J.S., Moore, C.D., & Dasgupta, N. (2017). Stereotype Threat and Stereotype Inoculation: Barriers and Interventions that Promote the Success of Underrepresented Students in the First Year of College. In R. Feldman (Ed.), First Year Student Success. Cambridge University Press. Denton, M., Borrego, M., & Boklage, A. (2020). Community cultural wealth in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics education: A systematic review. Journal of Engineering Education, 109(3), 556-580. Dickens, D. D., & Chavez, E. L. (2018). Navigating the workplace: The costs and benefits of shifting identities at work among early career US Black women. Sex Roles, 78(11), 760-774. Dixon, A.R., & Telles, E.E. (2017). Skin Color and Colorism: Global Research, Concepts, and Measurement, Annual Review of Sociology, 43, 1â20. Dolan E, Johnson D. (2009). Toward a holistic view of undergraduate research experiences: an exploratory study of impact on Graduate/Postdoctoral mentors. Journal of Science Education Technology. 18(6):487â500. Donahoo, S. (2022). Working with style: Black women, black hair, and professionalism. Gender, Work & Organization. Douglas, D., & Attewell, P. (2014). The bridge and the troll underneath: Summer bridge programs and degree completion. American Journal of Education, 121(1), 87-109. Drew, J. C., & Triplett, E. W. (2008). Whole genome sequencing in the undergraduate classroom: outcomes and lessons from a pilot course. Journal of Microbiology & Biology Education: JMBE, 9(1), 3. Duckworth, A. L., Peterson, C., Matthews, M. D., & Kelly, D. R. (2007). Grit: perseverance and passion for long-term goals. Journal of personality and social psychology, 92(6), 1087. Dunbar, R., & Barrett, L. (2007). Evolutionary psychology in the round. In L. Barrett & R. Dunbar Oxford, Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology â Oxford Handbook: Oxford University Press. Eberhardt, J.L., Davies, P.G., Purdie-Vaughns, V.J., & Johnson, S.L. (2006). Looking Deathworthy: Perceived Stereotypicality of Black Defendants Predicts Capital-Sentencing Outcomes, Psychological Science, 17(5), 383-386. Eidum, J., Lomicka, L., Chiang, W., Endick, G., & Stratton, J. (2020). Thriving in Residential Learning Communities. Learning Communities: Research & Practice, 8(1), 7. Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2005). Why it hurts to be left out: the neurocognitive overlap between physical and social pain. In K. D. Williams, J. P. Forgas, & W. von Hippel (Eds.). The social outcast: ostracism, social exclusion, rejection, and bullying (pp. 109-127). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Emerson, K. T. U., & Murphy, M. C. (2014). Identity threat at work: How social identity threat and situational cues contribute to racial and ethnic disparities in the workplace. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 20(4), 508â520. English, D., Lambert, S. F., Evans, M. K., & Zonderman, A. B. (2014). Neighborhood racial composition, racial discrimination, and depressive symptoms in African Americans. American journal of community psychology, 54(3), 219-228. Estrada, M. Flores, L. Young, G., Hernandez, P.R. & DeerInWater, K. (2022). Culture and Quality Matter in Building Effective Mentorship Relationships with Native STEM Scholars. Bioscience, biac064, https://doi.org/10.1093/biosci/biac064 Estrada, M., Hernandez, P., & Schultz, W. (2018). A longitudinal study of how quality mentorship and research experience integrate underrepresented minorities into STEM careers. CBE-Life Science Education, 17(1), pii: ar9. PMID: 29351912. 5-41 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
Estrada, M., Woodcock, A., Hernandez, P. R., & Schultz, P. W. (2011). Toward a model of social influence that explains minority student integration into the scientific community. J Educ Psychol, 103(1), 206-222. doi:10.1037/a0020743. Estrada, M., Young, G. R., Flores, L., Yu, B., & Matsui, J. (2021). Content and quality of science training programs matter: Longitudinal study of the Biology Scholars Program. CBEâLife Sciences Education, 20(3), ar44. Ewing, K. M., Richardson, T. Q., James-Myers, L., & Russell, R.K. (1996). The relationship between racial identity attitudes, worldview, and African American graduate studentsâ experience of the imposter phenomenon. Journal of Black Psychology, 22, 53â66. Ferguson Jr, M. W., & Dougherty, D. S. (2021). The paradox of the black professional: Whitewashing blackness through professionalism. Management Communication Quarterly, 08933189211019751. Fernández, Ã., Rincón, B. E., & Hinojosa, J. K. (2021). (Re) creating family and reinforcing pedagogies of the home: How familial capital manifests for Students of Color pursuing STEM majors. Race Ethnicity and Education, 1-17. Fischer, M. J. (2010). A longitudinal examination of the role of stereotype threat and racial climate on college outcomes for minorities at elite institutions. Social Psychology of Education: An International Journal, 13(1), 19â40. Fiske, S. T. (2005). Social Cognition and the Normality of Prejudgment. In J.F. Dovidio, P. Glick, & L.A. Rudman (Eds.). On the nature of prejudice: Fifty years after Allport (pp. 36-53). Malden: Blackwell Publishing. Fiske, S. T. (2009). From dehumanization and objectification to rehumanization. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1167(1), 31-34. Fiske, S.T., Dupre, C.H., Nicolas, G. , Swencionis, J.K. Status, Power, and Intergroup Relations: The Personal Is the Societal. Curr Opin Psychol. 2016 Oct;11:44-48. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.05.012. PMID: 27453923; PMCID: PMC4955850. Flores, D., Fondern, A., Ortiz, D., & Quinteros, K. (2018, February). Finding your STEM familia: Lessons from SACNAS. In Iowa State Conference on Race and Ethnicity (Vol. 19, No. 1). Forman,R. (2020) Yale's Colón-Ramos Energizes His Native Puerto Rico's Response to COVID-19. https://medicine.yale.edu/news-article/yales-colon-ramos-energizes-his-native-puerto-ricos-response- to-covid-19/ Frost, D.M. (2011). Social Stigma and its Consequences for the Socially Stigmatized. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 5(11), 824-839 Fuesting, M. A., & Diekman, A. B. (2017). Not by success alone: Role models provide pathways to communal opportunities in STEM. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43(2), 163-176. Gazley, J. L., & Campbell, P. B. (2020). The role of resilience in Black menâs success in STEM graduate programs. The Journal of Negro Education, 89(3), 360-372. Geronimus, A. T. (2001). Understanding and eliminating racial inequalities in womenâs health in the United States: the role of the weathering conceptual framework. JAMA, 56(4), 133e136, 149e150. Geronimus, A. T., Hicken, M., Keene, D., & Bound, J. (2006). âWeatheringâ and age patterns of allostatic load scores among blacks and whites in the United States. American Journal of Public Health, 96(5), 826e833 Gewin V. The hashtags that brought Black scientists together. Nature. 2021 Aug;596(7872):449-452. doi: 10.1038/d41586-021-02223-0. PMID: 34400829. Gibson, D. E. (2004). Role models in career development: New directions for theory and research. Journal of vocational behavior, 65(1), 134-156. Gilbert, P. A., & Zemore, S. E. (2016). Discrimination and drinking: A systematic review of the evidence. Social Science & Medicine, 161, 178-194. Ginther, D. K., Schaffer, W. T., Schnell, J., Masimore, B., Liu, F., Haak, L. L., & Kington, R. (2011). Race, ethnicity, and NIH research awards. Science, 333(6045), 1015-1019. 5-42 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
Gonzales, L.D., Hall, K., Benton, A. et al. Comfort over Change: a Case Study of Diversity and Inclusivity Efforts in U.S. Higher Education. Innov High Educ 46, 445â460 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10755-020-09541-7 Goffman, E. (1963). Stigma: Notes on the Management of Spoiled Identity. Simon & Schuster. Gonzalez, E. (2021). An Asset-Based Approach to Advancing Latina Students in STEM, Increasing Resilience, Participation, and Success. Retrieved on January 13, 2023 from https://faseb.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1096/fasebj.2021.35.S1.04522 González-Espada, W., Llerandi-Román, P., Fortis-Santiago, Y., Guerrero-Medina, G., Ortiz-Vega, N., Feliú-Mójer, M., & Colón-Ramos, D. (2015). Impact of culturally relevant contextualized activities on elementary and middle school studentsâ perceptions of science: An exploratory study. International Journal of Science Education, Part B, 5(2), 182-202. Griffith, E., & Dasgupta, N. (2018). How the demographic composition of academic science and engineering departments influences workplace culture, faculty experience, and retention risk. Social Sciences, 7, 71. Griffith, E.E., Mickey, E.L., & Dasgupta. N. (2022). A âChillierâ Climate for Multiply Marginalized STEM Faculty Impedes Research Collaboration. Sex Roles. Advance online publication. Grundy, S. M., Brewer, H. B., Jr., Cleeman, J. I., Smith, S. C., Jr., & Lenfant, C. (2004). Definition of metabolic syndrome: report of the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute/American Heart Association conference on scientific issues related to definition. Arteriosclerosis, Thrombosis, and Vascular Biology, 24(2), e13ee18. Guerrero-Medina, G., Feliú-Mójer, M., González-Espada, W., DÃaz-Muñoz, G., López, M., DÃaz-Muñoz, S. L., ... & Colón-Ramos, D. A. (2013). Supporting diversity in science through social networking. PLoS biology, 11(12), e1001740. Andrew Gulliford; Bones of Contention: The Repatriation of Native American Human Remains. The Public Historian 1 October 1996; 18 (4): 119â143. doi: https://doi.org/10.2307/3379790 Hagopian, A., West, K. M., Ornelas, I. J., Hart, A. N., Hagedorn, J., & Spigner, C. (2018). Adopting an anti-racism public health curriculum competency: the University of Washington experience. Public Health Reports, 133(4), 507-513. Haines, E. L., & Kray, L. J. (2005). Selfâpower associations: the possession of power impacts women's selfâconcepts. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35(5), 643-662. Hanauer, D. I., Frederick, J., Fotinakes, B., & Strobel, S. A. (2012). Linguistic analysis of project ownership for undergraduate research experiences. CBE-Life Sciences Education, 11(4), 378-385. Hathaway, R. S., Nagda, B. A., & Gregerman, S. R. (2002). The relationship of undergraduate research participation to graduate and professional education pursuit: An empirical study. Journal of College Student Development, 43(5), 614-631. Hernandez, P. R., Estrada, M., Woodcock, A., & Schultz, P. W. (2017). Protégé perceptions of high mentorship quality depend on shared values more than on demographic match. The Journal of Experimental Education, 85(3), 450-468. Hernandez, P. R., Woodcock, A., Estrada, M., & Schultz, P. W. (2018). Undergraduate research experiences broaden diversity in the scientific workforce. BioScience, 68(3), 204-211. Herrmann, S. D., Adelman, R. M., Bodford, J. E., Graudejus, O., Okun, M. A., & Kwan, V. S. (2016). The effects of a female role model on academic performance and persistence of women in STEM courses. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 38(5), 258-268. Hicks, D. (2011). Dignity: The essential role it plays in resolving conflict: Yale University Press. Higher Education Research Institute, 2010: Higher Education Research Institute at UCLA (HERI). 2010. Degrees of Success: Bachelorâs Degree Completion Rates Among Initial STEM Majors, HERI Report Brief. Available at http://www.heri.ucla.edu/nih/HERI_ResearchBrief_OL_2010_STEM.pdf Hilton, J.L. & Von Hippel, W. (1996). Stereotypes. Annual Review of Psychology, 47:237â71 Hirshfield, L. E., & Joseph, T. D. (2012). âWe need a woman, we need a black womanâ: Gender, race, and identity taxation in the academy. Gender and Education, 24(2), 213-227. 5-43 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
Hoyt, C. L., & Blascovich, J. (2007). Leadership efficacy and women leaders' responses to stereotype activation. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 10(4), 595-616. Hoyt, C. L., Aguilar, L., Kaiser, C. R., Blascovich, J., & Lee, K. (2007). The self-protective and undermining effects of attributional ambiguity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(6), 884-893. Hurtado, S., & Carter, D. F. (1997). Effects of college transition and perceptions of the campus racial climate on Latino college students' sense of belonging. Sociology of education, 324-345. Hurtado, S., Cabrera, N. L., Lin, M. H., Arellano, L., & Espinosa, L. L. (2009). Diversifying science: Underrepresented student experiences in structured research programs. Research in Higher Education, 50(2), 189-214. Inkelas, K. K., Daver, Z. E., Vogt, K. E., & Leonard, J. B. (2007). Livingâlearning programs and first- generation college studentsâ academic and social transition to college. Research in Higher education, 48(4), 403-434. Inzlicht, M., & Ben-Zeev, T. (2003). Do High-Achieving Female Students Underperform in Private? The Implications of Threatening Environments on Intellectual Processing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(4), 796. Isen, A. M. (2008). Some ways in which positive affect influences decision making and problem solving. In M. Lewis, J. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (Vol. 3, pp. 548-573). New York, NY: The Gulford Press. Jack, A.A. (2016) (No) harm in asking: Class, acquired cultural capital, and academic engagement at an elite university. Sociology of Education, 89(1), 1â19. Jack, A.A. (2019) The privileged poor: How elite colleges are failing disadvantaged students. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Javed, Z., Haisum Maqsood, M., Yahya, T., Amin, Z., Acquah, I., Valero-Elizondo, J., ... & Nasir, K. (2022). Race, racism, and cardiovascular health: Applying a social determinants of health framework to racial/ethnic disparities in cardiovascular disease. Circulation: Cardiovascular Quality and Outcomes, 15(1), e007917. Johnson, A., Brown, J., Carlone, H., & Cuevas, A. K. (2011). Authoring identity amidst the treacherous terrain of science: A multiracial feminist examination of the journeys of three women of color in science. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 48(4), 339-366. Johnson, D. R. (2011). Women of color in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM). New Directions for Institutional Research, 2011(152), 75-85. Johnson, T. A., & Bankhead, T. (2014). Hair it is: Examining the experiences of Black women with natural hair. Jones, S. C. T., & Neblett, E. W., Jr. (2019). The impact of racism on the mental health of people of color. In M. T. Williams, D. C. Rosen, & J. W. Kanter (Eds.), Eliminating race-based mental health disparities: Promoting equity and culturally responsive care across settings (pp. 79â97). Context Press/New Harbinger Publications. Jordan, T. C., Burnett, S. H., Carson, S., Caruso, S. M., Clase, K., DeJong, R. J., . . . Elgin, S. C.(2014). A broadly implementable research course in phage discovery and genomics for first-year undergraduate students. MBio, 5(1), e01051-01013. Keltner, D., & Lerner, J. S. (2010). Emotion. In S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (pp. 317-351). King, K. R. (2003). Racism or sexism? Attributional ambiguity and simultaneous memberships in multiple oppressed groups. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33(2), 223â247 Koch, L. M., Gross, A. M., & Kolts, R. (2001). Attitudes toward Black English and code switching. Journal of Black psychology, 27(1), 29-42. Kumar, S.,&Jagacinski, C. M. (2006). Imposters have goals too: The imposter phenomenon and its relationship to achievement goal theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 147â157. Lane, T. B., & Id-Deen, L. (2020). Nurturing the capital within: A qualitative investigation of black women and girls in STEM summer programs. Urban Education, 0042085920926225. 5-44 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
Leaper, C. (2011). More similarities than differences in contemporary theories of social development?: A plea for theory bridging. Advances in child development and behavior, 40, 337-378. Learning English (2018). Retrieved from https://learningenglish.voanews.com/a/puerto-rico-rebuilding- may-help-improve-science-education/4225170.html Lee Bitsoi, L., & Lowe, S. C. (2018). Journey into the sciences: Successful Native American STEM programs. In S. Waterman, S. Lowe & H. Shotton (Eds.), Beyond access: Indigenizing programs for Native American student success (pp. 87-106). Sterling, VA: Stylus Publishing LLC Lee Bitsoi, L., & Lowe, S. C. (2018). Journey into the sciences: Successful Native American STEM programs. In S. Waterman, S. Lowe & H. Shotton (Eds.), Beyond access: Indigenizing programs for Native American student success (pp. 87-106). Sterling, VA: Stylus Publishing LLC. Lewis, N. A., Jr., & Sekaquaptewa, D. (2016). Beyond test performance: A broader view of stereotype threat. Current Opinion in Psychology, 11, 40â43. Leyva, L. A. (2021). Black womenâs counter-stories of resilience and within-group tensions in the white, patriarchal space of mathematics education. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 52(2), 117-151. Linares, L. I. R., & Muñoz, S. M. (2011). Revisiting validation theory: Theoretical foundations, applications, and extensions. Enrollment Management Journal, 2(1), 12-33. Linn, M. C., Palmer, E., Baranger, A., Gerard, E., & Stone, E. (2015). Undergraduate research experiences: Impacts and opportunities. Science, 347(6222), 1261757. Lockwood, P. (2006). âSomeone like me can be successfulâ: Do college students need same-gender role models?. Psychology of women quarterly, 30(1), 36-46. Lockwood, P., & Kunda, Z. (1997). Superstars and me: Predicting the impact of role models on the self. Journal of personality and social psychology, 73(1), 91. Lockwood, P., & Kunda, Z. (1999). Increasing the salience of one's best selves can undermine inspiration by outstanding role models. Journal of personality and social psychology, 76(2), 214. Lockwood, P., Jordan, C. H., & Kunda, Z. (2002). Motivation by positive or negative role models: regulatory focus determines who will best inspire us. Journal of personality and social psychology, 83(4), 854. Lopatto, D., Alvarez, C., Barnard, D., Chandrasekaran, C., Chung, H.-M., Du, C., . . . Jones, C. J. (2008). Genomics education partnership. Science, 322(5902), 684-685. MacArthur, B. L., Lindenfeld, L. A., Aurbach, E. L., Bevan, B., & Newman, T. P. (2020). Bridging Science with Society: Defining Pathways for Engagement. Communication Center Journal, 6(1), 62- 78. Macrae CN, & Bodenhausen GV. 2000. Social cognition: thinking categorically about others. Annual Review of Psychology, 51:93â120 Maddox KB, & Perry JM. (2018). Racial Appearance Bias: Improving Evidence-Based Policies to Address Racial Disparities. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2018;5(1):57-65. doi:10.1177/2372732217747086 Madubata, I., Spivey, L. A., Alvarez, G. M., Neblett, E. W., & Prinstein, M. J. (2022). Forms of racial/ethnic discrimination and suicidal ideation: A prospective examination of African-American and Latinx youth. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 51(1), 23-31. Major, B., & Crocker, J. (1993). Social stigma: The consequences of attributional ambiguity. In D. M. Mackie & D. L. Hamilton (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and stereotyping: Interactive processes in group perception (pp. 345â370). Academic Press. Major, B., & OâBrien, L. T. (2005). The Social Psychology of Stigma. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 393-421. Major, B., Quinton, W. J., & McCoy, S. K. (2002). Antecedents and consequences of attributions to discrimination: Theoretical and empirical advances. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 34, pp. 251â330). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065- 2601(02)80007-7 5-45 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
Major, B., & Schmader, T. (1998). Coping with stigma through psychological disengagement. In Prejudice (pp. 219-241). Academic Press. Major, B., Feinstein, J., & Crocker, J. (1994). Attributional ambiguity of affirmative action. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 15(1-2), 113â141 Marker, M. (2015). Indigenous knowledge, indigenous scholars, and narrating scientific selves: âTo produce a human beingâ. Cultural Studies of Science Education, 11, 477-480. doi: 10.1007/s11422- 015-9660-1 Martin, J. P., Revelo, R. A., Stefl, S. K., Garrett, S. D., & Adams, S. G. (2016, June). Ethnic student organizations in engineering: Implications for practice from two studies. In 2016 ASEE Annual Conference & Exposition Marx, D. M., & Roman, J. S. (2002). Female role models: Protecting womenâs math test performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(9), 1183-1193. Marx, D. M., Stapel, D. A., & Muller, D. (2005). We can do it: the interplay of construal orientation and social comparisons under threat. Journal of personality and social psychology, 88(3), 432. Maslow, A. (1943). A theory of human motivation. Psychological review, 50(4), 370. Maslow, A. (1968). Toward a Psychology of Being: Van Reinhold. Masten, A. S., & Narayan, A. J. (2012). Child development in the context of disaster, war, and terrorism: Pathways of risk and resilience. Annual review of psychology, 63, 227. McArdle, E. (2008). The freedom to say âno.â Retrieved from http://www.boston.com/bostonglobe/ideas/articles/2008/05/18/ the freedom to say no/?page=4 McCluney, C. L., Durkee, M. I., Smith II, R. E., Robotham, K. J., & Lee, S. S. L. (2021). To be, or not to be⦠Black: The effects of racial codeswitching on perceived professionalism in the workplace. Journal of experimental social psychology, 97, 104199. McCluney, Robotham, Lee, Smith, & Durkee, 2019. The Costs of Code-Switching. https://hbr.org/2019/11/the-costs-of-codeswitching . Harvard Business Review McCoy, D. L., Winkle-Wagner, R., & Luedke, C. L. (2015). Colorblind mentoring? Exploring white faculty mentoring of students of color. Journal of Diversity in Higher Education, 8, 225â242. doi:10.1037/a0038676 McDade, T. W., & Hayward, M. D. (2009). Rationale and methodological options for assessing infectious disease and related measures in social science surveys. Biodemography and Social Biology, 55(2), 159e177. McDade, T. W., Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2006). Psychosocial and behavioral predictors of inflammation in middle-aged and older adults: the Chicago health, aging, and social relations study. Psychosomatic medicine, 68(3), 376-381. McGee R. (2016). Biomedical Workforce Diversity: The Context for Mentoring to Develop Talents and Foster Success Within the 'Pipeline'. AIDS and behavior, 20 Suppl 2, 231â237. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10461-016-1486-7 McGee, E. O. (2016). Devalued Black and Latino racial identities: A by-product of STEM college culture? American Educational Research Journal, 53(6), 1626-1662. McGee, E. O., & Bentley, L. (2017). The troubled success of Black women in STEM. Cognition and Instruction, 35(4), 265-289. McGregor, L., Gee, D., & Posey, K. (2008). I feel like a fraud and it depresses me: The relation between the imposter phenomenon and depression. Social Behavior and Personality, 36, 43â48. Melamed, S., Shirom, A., Toker, S., Berliner, S., & Shapira, I. (2006). Burnout and risk of cardiovascular disease: evidence, possible causal paths, and promising research directions. Psychological Bulletin, 132(3), 327e353 Mendes, W. B., Blascovich, J., Major, B., & Seery, M. (2001). Challenge and threat responses during downward and upward social comparisons. European Journal of Social Psychology, 31(5), 477-497. Mendes, W. B., Major, B., McCoy, S., & Blascovich, J. (2008). How attributional ambiguity shapes physiological and emotional responses to social rejection and acceptance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(2), 278â291. 5-46 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
Mendoza-Denton, R., Downey, G., Purdie, V. J., Davis, A., & Pietrzak, J. (2002). Sensitivity to status- based rejection: Implications for African American students' college experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(4), 896â918. Mendoza-Denton, R., Pietrzak, J., & Downey, G. (2008). Distinguishing institutional identification from academic goal pursuit: Interactive effects of ethnic identification and race-based rejection sensitivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(2), 338â351. Miles, M.L., Brockman, A.J. Naphan-Kingery, D.E. (2020). Invalidated identities: The disconfirming effects of racial microaggressions on Black doctoral students in STEM. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 57(10), 1529-1651 Mishra, S. (2020) Social networks, social capital, social support and academic success in higher education: A systematic review with a special focus on âunderrepresentedâ students. Educational Research Review, 29, 100307 Misra, J., Smith-Doerr, L., Dasgupta, N., Weaver, G., & Normanly, J. (2017). Collaboration and gender equity among academic scientists. Social Sciences, 6, 25. Mondisa, J. L., Packard, B. W. L., & Montgomery, B. L. (2021). Understanding what STEM mentoring ecosystems need to thrive: A STEM-ME framework. Mentoring & Tutoring: Partnership in Learning, 29(1), 110-135. Morales, Walker, Carroll-Smith, & Burt (2021). Code-Switching and Assimiliatino in STEM Culture. https://eos.org/opinions/code-switching-and-assimilation-in-stem-culture . Eos Science News by AGU. Morton, T. R., & Nkrumah, T. (2021). A day of reckoning for the white academy: Reframing success for African American women in STEM. Cultural Studies of Science Education, 16(2), 485-494. Morton, T. R., & Parsons, E. C. (2018). # BlackGirlMagic: The identity conceptualization of Black women in undergraduate STEM education. Science Education, 102(6), 1363-1393. Morzinski, J. A., & Fisher, J. C. (2002). A nationwide study of the influence of faculty development programs on colleague relationships. Academic Medicine, 77(5), 402-406. Muller CB, Blake-Beard S, Barsion S, Wotipka CM. (2012). Learning from the experiences of women of color in MentorNetâs one-on-one program. Journal of Women and Minorities in Science and Engineering, 18(4), 317â38. Nagda, B. A., Gregerman, S. R., Jonides, J., Von Hippel, W., & Lerner, J. S. (1998). Undergraduate student-faculty research partnerships affect student retention. The Review of Higher Education, 22(1), 55-72. National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. (2020). The science of effective mentorship in STEMM. National Academies Press. National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2011. Expanding Underrepresented Minority Participation: America's Science and Technology Talent at the Crossroads. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. https://doi.org/10.17226/12984. National Society for Black Engineers. (n.d.). History. Retrieved January 12, 2023, from https://www.nsbe.org/About-Us/History Nguyen, T., Cho, Y. J., & Jang, Y. (2021). Perceived discrimination, psychosocial resources, and mental distress in Vietnamese Americans. Journal of Migration and Health, 3, 100039. Nirmal. N.A (2021) Taking Care of Each Other, with Science and Solidarity. https://www.simonsfoundation.org/2021/04/19/taking-care-of-each-other-with-science-and-solidarity/ Nussbaum, A. D., & Steele, C. M. (2007). Situational disengagement and persistence in the face of adversity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(1), 127â 134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2005.12.007 OâReilly, K. B. (2020). AMA: racism is a threat to public health. American Medical Association. O'Brien, K.R., McAbee, S.T., Hebl, M.R., Rodgers, J.R. (2016) The impact of interpersonal discrimination and stress on health and performance for early career STEM academicians. Frontiers in Psychology 5-47 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
Ondrechen, M. J. (2014). American Indian Science and Engineering Society (AISES): Building a Successful Model for Diversity and Inclusion. In Careers, Entrepreneurship, and Diversity: Challenges and Opportunities in the Global Chemistry Enterprise (pp. 255-264). American Chemical Society. Ong, M. (2005). Body projects of young women of color in physics: Intersections of gender, race, and science. Social problems, 52(4), 593-617. Opie, T. R., & Phillips, K. W. (2015). Hair penalties: The negative influence of Afrocentric hair on ratings of Black womenâs dominance and professionalism. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1311. Padilla, A. M. (1994). Research news and comment: Ethnic minority scholars; research, and mentoring: Current and future issues. Educational researcher, 23(4), 24-27. Page-Reeves, J., Cortez, G. L., Ortiz, Y., Moffett, M., Deerinwater, K., & Medin, D. (2019). Situating giving back for Native Americans pursuing careers in STEM:âYou donât just take, you give something backâ. Intersections: Critical Issues in Education, 3(1), 4. Panter-Brick, C., Layton, R., & Rowley-Conwy, P. (2001). Hunter-gatherers: an interdisciplinary perspective (Vol. 13): Cambridge University Press. Paradies, Y., Ben, J., Denson, N., Elias, A., Priest, N., Pieterse, A., ... & Gee, G. (2015). Racism as a determinant of health: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PloS one, 10(9), e0138511. Parks-Stamm, E. J., Heilman, M. E., & Hearns, K. A. (2008). Motivated to penalize: Women's strategic rejection of successful women. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(2), 237-247. Perez, R., & Robnett, R. D. (2014). Thinking Like a Scientist: A Thematic Analysis of Students' Experiences at the SACNAS Research Conference. Pfund, C, Angela Byars-Winston, Janet Branchaw, Sylvia Hurtado, Kevin Eagan. (2016) Defining Attributes and Metrics of Effective Research Mentoring Relationships. AIDS and Behavior 20, S238â S248. DOI 10.1007/s10461-016-1384-z Pieterse, A. L., Todd, N. R., Neville, H. A., & Carter, R. T. (2012). Perceived racism and mental health among Black American adults: a meta-analytic review. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 59(1), 1. Pinker, S. (2008). The sexual paradox: Extreme men, gifted women and the real gender gap. Toronto, Canada: Random House. Plascak, Jesse J., et al. "Association Between Residence in Historically Redlined Districts Indicative of Structural Racism and Racial and Ethnic Disparities in Breast Cancer Outcomes." JAMA Network Open 5.7 (2022): e2220908-e2220908. Pradhan, A. D., Manson, J. E., Rifai, N., Buring, J. E., & Ridker, P. M. (2001). C-reactive protein, interleukin 6, and risk of developing type 2 diabetes mellitus. JAMA, 286(3), 327e33z Presidentâs Council of Advisors on Science and Technology. (2012). Engage to excel: Producing one million additional college graduates with degrees in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics. Retrieved September 2020, from www.whitehouse.gov/sites/default/files/microsites/ostp/pcast-engage-to-excel-final_feb.pdf Prunuske AJ, Wilson J, Walls M, Clarke B. (2013). Experiences of mentors training underrepresented undergraduates in the research laboratory. CBE Life Sci Educ. 12(3), 403â9. Pumaccahua, T., & Rogers, M. R. (2022). Academic Warriors: Community Cultural Wealth among Latinx and Black STEM Doctoral Students at Predominately White Institutions. Journal of Latinos and Education, 1-15. Purdie-Vaughns, V., Steele, C. M., Davies, P. G., Ditlmann, R., & Crosby, J. R. (2008). Social identity contingencies: how diversity cues signal threat or safety for African Americans in mainstream institutions. Journal of personality and social psychology, 94(4), 615. Rabelo, V. C., Robotham, K. J., & McCluney, C. L. (2021). âAgainst a sharp white backgroundâ: How Black women experience the white gaze at work. Gender, Work & Organization, 28(5), 1840-1858. Ramirez, G., Covarrubias, R., Jackson, M., & Son, J. Y. (2021). Making hidden resources visible in a minority serving college context. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 27(2), 256. Ray, V. (2019). A theory of racialized organizations. American Sociological Review, 84(1), 26-53. 5-48 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
Remedios, J. D., & Snyder, S. H. (2015). How women of color detect and respond to multiple forms of prejudice. Sex Roles: A Journal of Research, 73(9-10), 371â383. Ridker, P. M., Buring, J. E., Cook, N. R., & Rifai, N. (2003). C-reactive protein, the metabolic syndrome, and risk of incident cardiovascular events: an 8-year follow-up of 14 719 initially healthy American women. Circulation, 107(3), 391e397 Rincón, B. E., & Rodriguez, S. (2021). Latinx students charting their own STEM pathways: How community cultural wealth informs their STEM identities. Journal of Hispanic Higher Education, 20(2), 149-163. Rincón, B.E, Fernández, E & Dueñas, M.C (2020): Anchoring comunidad: how first- and continuing- generation Latinx students in STEM engage community cultural wealth, International Journal of Qualitative Studies in Education, DOI: 10.1080/09518398.2020.1735567 Rocha, J., Cabral, B., Landeros, J., & Yancy, C. W. (2022). Why Continuity of STEM-Medicine Participation Matters: Exploring a Culture of Transformation and the Optimization of College Socialization. Journal of Advanced Academics, 33(3), 433-468 Rockinson-Szapkiw, A., Watson, J. H., Gishbaugher, J., & Wendt, J. L. (2021). A case for a virtual STEM peer-mentoring experience for racial and ethnic minority women mentees. International Journal of Mentoring and Coaching in Education. Rodenbusch, S. E., Hernandez, P. R., Simmons, S. L., & Dolan, E. L. (2016). Early engagement in course-based research increases graduation rates and completion of science, engineering, and mathematics degrees. CBE-Life Sciences Education, 15(2), ar20. Rosenbloom, J. L., Ash, R. A., Dupont, B., & Coder, L. (2008).Why are there so few women in information technology? Assessing the role of personality in career choices. Journal of Economic Psychology, 29, 543â554. Ross, M. S., & McGrade, S. (2016, June). An exploration into the impacts of the National Society of Black Engineers (NSBE) on student persistence. In 2016 ASEE Annual Conference & Exposition. Ross, M. S., Huff, J. L., & Godwin, A. (2021). Resilient engineering identity development critical to prolonged engagement of Black women in engineering. Journal of Engineering Education, 110(1), 92-113. Rudman, L. A., & Phelan, J. E. (2010). The effect of priming gender roles on womenâs implicit gender beliefs and career aspirations. Social psychology, 41(3), 192. Russell, D. W., Clavél, F. D., Cutrona, C. E., Abraham, W. T., & Burzette, R. G. (2018). Neighborhood racial discrimination and the development of major depression. Journal of abnormal psychology, 127(2), 150. Sanchez, D., Smith, L. V., & Adams, W. (2018). The relationships among perceived discrimination, marianismo gender role attitudes, racial-ethnic socialization, coping styles, and mental health outcomes in Latina college students. Journal of Latina/o Psychology, 6(1), 1â15. Schmader, T., Johns,M., & Forbes, C. (2008). An integrated process model of stereotype threat effects on performance. Psychological Review, 115, 336â356. Schussler, D., & Fierros, E. (2008). Students' perceptions of their academics, relationships, and sense of belonging: Comparisons across residential learning communities. Journal of the First-Year Experience & Students in Transition, 20(1), 71-96. Sekaquaptewa, D., & Thompson, M. (2002). The differential effects of solo status on members of high- and low-status groups. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(5), 694-707. Sekaquaptewa, D., Waldman, A., & Thompson, M. (2007). Solo status and self-construal: being distinctive influences racial self-construal and performance apprehension in African American women. Cultural diversity & ethnic minority psychology, 13(4), 321â327. https://doi.org/10.1037/1099-9809.13.4.321 Shaffer, C. D., Alvarez, C. J., Bednarski, A. E., Dunbar, D., Goodman, A. L., Reinke, C., . . . Barnard, D. (2014). A course-based research experience: how benefits change with increased investment in instructional time. CBE-Life Sciences Education, 13(1), 111-130. 5-49 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
Shaffer, C. D., Alvarez, C., Bailey, C., Barnard, D., Bhalla, S., Chandrasekaran, C., . . . Du, C. (2010). The Genomics Education Partnership: successful integration of research into laboratory classes at a diverse group of undergraduate institutions. CBE-Life Sciences Education, 9(1), 55-69. Shapiro, N. S., & Levine, J. H. (1999). Creating Learning Communities: A Practical Guide to Winning Support, Organizing for Change, and Implementing Programs. Jossey-Bass Higher and Adult Education Series. Jossey-Bass Inc., Publishers, 350 Sansome St., San Francisco, CA 94104. Sherif, M. (1988). The robbers cave experiment: Intergroup conflict and cooperation.[Orig. pub. as Intergroup conflict and group relations]. Wesleyan University Press. Simmons, O.S. (2011) Lost in translation: The implications of social capital for higher education access. Notre Dame Law Review, 87(1), 205â252. Slopen, N., Lewis, T. T., & Williams, D. R. (2016). Discrimination and sleep: a systematic review. Sleep medicine, 18, 88-95. Smith, J. L., Metz, A., Cech, E., Hunatoon, M., & Moyer, C. (2014). Giving back or giving up: Native American student experiences in science and engineering. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 20(3), 413-429. doi: 10.1037/a0036945 Smith, T.D. (2022), unpublished. Natives in STEM Literature Review. Paper commissioned by the Committee on Advancing Antiracism, Diversity, Equity, and Inclusion in STEMM Organizations, National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine, Washington, DC. Smith, R. E. II, & Wout, D. A. (2019). Blacksâ perception of a Biracialâs ingroup membership shapes attributions to discrimination following social rejection. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 25(4), 483â493. Society for Chicanos, Hispanics, and Natie Americans in Science. (n.d.). History of SACNAS. Retrieved January 12, 2023, from https://www.sacnas.org/history-of-sacnas Soldner, M., Rowan-Kenyon, H., Inkelas, K. K., Garvey, J., & Robbins, C. (2012). Supporting students' intentions to persist in STEM disciplines: The role of living-learning programs among other social- cognitive factors. The Journal of Higher Education, 83(3), 311-336. Solomon, T. G. A., Starks, R. R. B., Attakai, A., Molina, F., Cordova-Marks, F., Kahn-John, M., ... & Garcia, F. (2022). The Generational Impact Of Racism On Health: Voices From American Indian Communities: Study examines the generational impact of racism on the health of American Indian communities and people. Health Affairs, 41(2), 281-288. Soso, S.B (2021). Retrieved from https://www.includesnetwork.org/blogs/nsf-includes-coordination- hub1/2021/05/19/social-media-and-stem-diversity-how-minority-docto Southwick, S. M., Bonanno, G. A., Masten, A. S., Panter-Brick, C., & Yehuda, R. (2014). Resilience definitions, theory, and challenges: interdisciplinary perspectives. European journal of psychotraumatology, 5(1), 25338. Sparks, D. M., Przymus, S. D., Silveus, A., De La Fuente, Y., & Cartmill, C. (2021). Navigating the intersectionality of race/ethnicity, culture, and gender identity as an aspiring Latina STEM student. Journal of Latinos and Education, 1-17. Spencer, Logel, & Davies (2016). Stereotype Threat. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 415-437 Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., & Quinn, D. M. (1999). Stereotype threat and women's math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35(1), 4â 28. https://doi.org/10.1006/jesp.1998.1373 Stanley, C. A. (2006). Coloring the academic landscape: Faculty of color breaking the silence in predominantly White colleges and universities. American educational research journal, 43(4), 701- 736. Steele, C. M. (1988). The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the integrity of the self. Advances in experimental social psychology, 21, 261â302. Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air. How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance.American Psychologist, 52(6), 613â629. Steele, C. M. (1998). Stereotyping and its threat are real. American Psychologist, 53(6), 680-681. 5-50 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
Steele, C. M. (2011). Whistling Vivaldi: And other clues to how stereotypes affect us (issues of our time): WW Norton & Company. Steele, C. M., Spencer, S. J., & Aronson, J. (2002). Contending with group image: The psychology of stereotype and social identity threat. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 34, pp. 379â440). San Diego, CA: Academic Stein, G. L., Castro-Schilo, L., Cavanaugh, A. M., Mejia, Y., Christophe, N. K., & Robins, R. (2019). When discrimination hurts: The longitudinal impact of increases in peer discrimination on anxiety and depressive symptoms in Mexican-origin youth. Journal of youth and adolescence, 48(5), 864- 875. Stephens, N. M., Hamedani, M. G., & Destin, M. (2014). Closing the social-class achievement gap: A difference-education intervention improves first-generation studentsâ academic performance and all studentsâ college transition. Psychological science, 25(4), 943-953. Stephens, N.M., Fryberg, S.A., Markus, H.R., Johnson, C.S. & Covarrubias, R. (2012) Unseen disadvantage: How American universitiesâ focus on independence undermines the academic performance of first-generation college students. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(6), 1178â1197. Stevenson, A. D., Gallard MartÃnez, A. J., Brkich, K. L., Flores, B. B., Claeys, L., & Pitts, W. (2019). Latinasâ heritage language as a source of resiliency: Impact on academic achievement in STEM fields. Cultural Studies of Science Education, 14(1), 1-13. Stout, J. G., Dasgupta, N., Hunsinger, M., & McManus, M. A. (2011). STEMing the tide: using ingroup experts to inoculate women's self-concept in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM). Journal of personality and social psychology, 100(2), 255. Strayhorn, T. L. (2011). Bridging the pipeline: Increasing underrepresented studentsâ preparation for college through a summer bridge program. American Behavioral Scientist, 55(2), 142-159. Suzuki, A., Amrein-Beardsley, A., & Perry, N. (2012). A summer bridge program for underprepared first- year students: Confidence, community, and re-enrollment. Journal of the First-Year Experience & Students in Transition, 24(2), 85-106. Tajfel, H. (1978). Social Categorization, Social Identity and Social Comparison. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), Differentiation between Social Groups: Studies in the Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations (pp. 61-76). London: Academic Press. Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G. Austin, & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33-37). Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole. Thiem, K., & Dasgupta, N. (2022). From pre-college to career: Barriers facing historically marginalized students and research-driven solutions. Social Issues and Policy Review. Online first. Thomas, D. A. (2001). The truth about mentoring minorities. Race matters. Harvard business review, 79(4), 98-107. Thompson, M., & Sekaquaptewa, D. (2002). When Being Different Is Detrimental: Solo Status and the Performance of Women and Racial Minorities.. Analyses of Social Issues & Public Policy, 2(1), 183- 203. Thompson, T., Davis, H., & Davidson, J. (1998). Attributional and affective responses of impostors to academic success and failure outcomes. Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 381â396. Topping, M. E.,&Kimmel, E. B. (1985). The imposter phenomenon: Feeling phony. Academic Psychology Bulletin, 7, 213â226. Townsend, S. S., Stephens, N. M., Smallets, S., & Hamedani, M. G. (2019). Empowerment through difference: An online difference-education intervention closes the social class achievement gap. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 45(7), 1068-1083. Utsey, S. O., Hook, J. N., Fischer, N., & Belvet, B. (2008). Cultural orientation, ego resilience, and optimism as predictors of subjective well-being in African Americans. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 3(3), 202-210. 5-51 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs
Walton, G. M., & Cohen, G. L. (2007). A question of belonging: race, social fit, and achievement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(1), 82. Walton, G. M., & Cohen, G. L. (2011). A brief social-belonging intervention improves academic and health outcomes of minority students. Science, 331(6023), 1447â 1451. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1198364 Walton, G. M., & Cohen, G. L. (2011). Outcomes of minority students: A brief social-belonging intervention improves academic and health outcomes. Science, 331, 1447-1451. Wawrzynski, M. R., Jessup-Anger, J., Stolz, K., Helman, C., & Beaulieu, J. (2009). Exploring students' perceptions of academically based living-learning communities. College Student Affairs Journal. Weaver, G. C., Russell, C. B., & Wink, D. J. (2008). Inquiry-based and research-based laboratory pedagogies in undergraduate science. Nature chemical biology, 4(10), 577-580. Weinstein, M., Vaupel, J. W., & Wachter, K. W. (2007). Biosocial surveys. Washington, D.C.: National Academies Press. Wester, S.R., Vogel, D.L., Wei, M., & McLain, R. (2011). African American Men, Gender Role Conflict, and Psychological Distress: The Role of Racial Identity. Journal of Counseling and Development, 84(4), 419-429. Williams, D. R., Lawrence, J. A., Davis, B. A., & Vu, C. (2019). Understanding how discrimination can affect health. Health services research, 54, 1374-1388. Williams, D. R., Lawrence, J., & Davis, B. (2019). Racism and health: evidence and needed research. Annual review of public health, 40, 105. Wolfe, C. T., & Spencer, S. J. (1996). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their overt and subtle influence in the classroom. American Behavioral Scientist, 40(2), 176â 185. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002764296040002008 Woodcock, A., Hernandez, P. R., Estrada, M., & Schultz, P. W. (2012). The consequences of chronic stereotype threat: domain disidentification and abandonment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103(4). doi:10.1037/a0029120. Wu, D., Thiem, K., & Dasgupta, N. (accepted in principle). The enduring impact of female peer mentors in the first year of college on women students in engineering from college entry through graduation. Nature Communication. Wu, D., Ziegenbein, L., Phillis, R., Zehnder, C., Connor, E., Gibson, T., & Dasgupta, N. (in preparation). The effect of a learning community on first-generation studentsâ success in biology. Unpublished manuscript. University of Massachusetts. Yosso, T. J. (2005). Whose culture has capital? A critical race theory discussion of community cultural wealth. Race ethnicity and education, 8(1), 69-91. Yudkin, J. S. (2003). Adipose tissue, insulin action and vascular disease: inflammatory signals. International Journal of Obesity, 27(Suppl. 3), S25eS28. Zambrana, R. E., Ray, R., Espino, M. M., Castro, C., Douthirt Cohen, B., & Eliason, J. (2015). âDonât leave us behindâ The importance of mentoring for underrepresented minority faculty. American Educational Research Journal, 52(1), 40-72. 5-52 Pre-Publication Copy, Uncorrected Proofs