National Academies Press: OpenBook

Emergent Techniques for Assessment of Visual Performance (1985)

Chapter: APPENDIX A: BASIC FACTORS IN SPATIAL CONTRAST SENSITIVITY

« Previous: SUMMARY RECOMMENDATIONS
Suggested Citation:"APPENDIX A: BASIC FACTORS IN SPATIAL CONTRAST SENSITIVITY." National Research Council. 1985. Emergent Techniques for Assessment of Visual Performance. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/916.
×
Page 31
Suggested Citation:"APPENDIX A: BASIC FACTORS IN SPATIAL CONTRAST SENSITIVITY." National Research Council. 1985. Emergent Techniques for Assessment of Visual Performance. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/916.
×
Page 32
Suggested Citation:"APPENDIX A: BASIC FACTORS IN SPATIAL CONTRAST SENSITIVITY." National Research Council. 1985. Emergent Techniques for Assessment of Visual Performance. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/916.
×
Page 33
Suggested Citation:"APPENDIX A: BASIC FACTORS IN SPATIAL CONTRAST SENSITIVITY." National Research Council. 1985. Emergent Techniques for Assessment of Visual Performance. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/916.
×
Page 34
Suggested Citation:"APPENDIX A: BASIC FACTORS IN SPATIAL CONTRAST SENSITIVITY." National Research Council. 1985. Emergent Techniques for Assessment of Visual Performance. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/916.
×
Page 35
Suggested Citation:"APPENDIX A: BASIC FACTORS IN SPATIAL CONTRAST SENSITIVITY." National Research Council. 1985. Emergent Techniques for Assessment of Visual Performance. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/916.
×
Page 36

Below is the uncorrected machine-read text of this chapter, intended to provide our own search engines and external engines with highly rich, chapter-representative searchable text of each book. Because it is UNCORRECTED material, please consider the following text as a useful but insufficient proxy for the authoritative book pages.

APPENDIX A: BASIC FACTORS IN SPATIAL CONTRAST SENSITIVITY 31 APPENDIX A: BASIC FACTORS IN SPATIAL CONTRAST SENSITIVITY The dependent variable that is the basis of the contrast sensitivity function is the contrast of the sinusoidal test grating. The most commonly used definition of contrast is: where Lmax is the maximum luminance at the peak and Lmin is the minimum luminance at the trough of the sine wave. Contrast may vary from 0.0 (a field of uniform luminance) to 1.0 (where Lmin = 0.0 and Lmax = 2.0 * Lmean since Lmean = (Lmax + Lmin)/2). It is not possible to have a real sinusoidal grating of contrast greater than 1.0, because luminance cannot be less than 0.0. At this time there are no standards for the measurement of the contrast sensitivity function, although there is general understanding of the factors that influence its shape. These factors are discussed below. MEAN LUMINANCE The mean luminance of the sinusoidal gratings has a profound effect on the contrast sensitivity function. At high photopic levels of mean luminance, the normal contrast sensitvity function has a peak sensitivity at about 5 cpd and a high frequency cutoff at about 60 cpd. As mean luminance is lowered, not only do the frequencies of the peak sensitivity and the high frequency cutoff become lower, but also the height of the peak is reduced (Figure 10). At mesopic levels of mean luminance the peak in the contrast sensitivity function has practically disappeared. This peak, which is so prominent at high luminances, is generally believed to reflect the dynamic interaction between excitatory and inhibitory influences in the visual system. RETINAL LOCUS Compared with the contrast sensitivity function measured at the fovea, the contrast sensitivity function measured for eccentric retinal loci shows a progressive shift to lower spatial frequencies for both the high frequency cutoff and the frequency of peak sensitivity (Figure 11). In addition there is a progressive lowering of overall contrast sensitivity. This change is believed to reflect the unequal way in which the retinal visual field is projected onto the visual cortex (Daniel and Whitteridge, 1961; Schwartz, 1980). When measured at different retinal eccentricities using sinusoidal gratings whose size and spatial frequency have been adjusted to stimulate equal amounts of visual cortex, the contrast sensitivity function is approximately the same at all retinal loci (Virsu and Rovamo, 1979; Rovamo and Virsu, 1979) as is shown in Figure 12.

APPENDIX A: BASIC FACTORS IN SPATIAL CONTRAST SENSITIVITY 32 FIGURE 10 Contrast sensitivity functions for different levels of mean luminance. The upper curves were measured with gratings having a mean luminance of 500 cd/m2. The lower curves were measured at 0.05 cd/m2. SOURCE: Campbell and Robson, 1968. Reprinted with permission from F. W. Campbell and J. G. Robson. Copyright 1968 by The Physiological Society. FIELD SIZE Visual sensitivity at a particular spatial frequency depends on how many cycles of the sine wave grating are included in the pattern. Sensitivity increases as more cycles are included up to about 10 complete cycles (Figure 13). Usually the low frequency limit of testing is limited by the largest possible field. For example, in order to measure maximum sensitivity at 0.5 cpd, one would need a test field 20 deg square to have 10 complete cycles. TEMPORAL CHARACTERISTICS Both the exposure duration of the grating target and the time course of its onset influence the contrast sensitivity function, especially at the lower spatial frequencies. Figure 14 shows the contrast sensitivity function measured with two different stimulus duration characteristics. Notice that with brief exposure duration or with rapid onset of the grating stimulus, sensitivity to low spatial frequencies is enhanced

APPENDIX A: BASIC FACTORS IN SPATIAL CONTRAST SENSITIVITY 33 compared with longer duration, more gradual onset stimuli. If gratings are modulated sinusoidally in time, the temporal frequency influences the measured contrast sensitivity function. The interaction between temporal frequency and spatial frequency are represented by a contrast sensitivity surface, as shown in Figure 15 (Kelly, 1979). Notice that contrast sensitivity at low spatial frequencies is higher when the grating is flickered at a relatively high temporal modulation compared with a low temporal modulation. FIGURE 11 A comparison of contrast sensitivity functions for the nasal visual-field half-meridian of the right eye (open symbols and continuous lines) and for the temporal half-meridian of the left eye (filled-in symbols and dashed lines). The values of eccentricity were measured as the angular distance of the fixation point from the middle of the gratings that subtended 10 deg. The nasal eccentricities were 0 deg (half-filled circles), 10 deg (triangles), 20 deg (diamonds), and 30 deg (squares). The temporal eccentricities were 0 deg (half-filled circles), 30 deg (filled squares), 50 deg (filled triangles), and 60 deg (filled circles). The foveal functions were similar for both eyes and are not drawn separately. SOURCE: Rovamo and Virsu, 1979. Reprinted with permission from J. Rovamo and V. Virsu. Copyright 1979 by Springer-Verlag.

APPENDIX A: BASIC FACTORS IN SPATIAL CONTRAST SENSITIVITY 34 FIGURE 12 The photopic contrast sensitivity functions for 25 locations of the visual field. The eccentricities were measured along the various half-meridians of the visual field as indicated on the graphs and in A for the different symbols. The retinal dimensions of the gratings were scaled for equivalent calculated cortical representations. Contrast sensitivity is shown as a function of spatial frequency in the calculated cortical projection images (cycles per millimeter of cortex). In contrast to the data in Figure 12, the contrast sensitivity functions at the different retinal eccentricities are almost identical. SOURCE: Rovamo and Virsu, 1979. Reprinted with permission from J. Rovamo and V. Virsu. Copyright 1979 by Springer-Verlag. ORIENTATION The visual system is more sensitive to horizontal and vertical gratings than to other orientations, an example of the oblique effect (Appelle, 1972) wherein horizontal and vertical orientations are more important in vision than are obliques. Figure 16 shows the contrast sensitivity function measured with vertical gratings and for 45 deg orientation gratings. The contrast sensitivity, especially for the high spatial frequencies, is reduced for oblique orientations relative to horizontal and vertical orientations. This orientation anisotropy may be important for calculations of the “effective” visual stimulus.

APPENDIX A: BASIC FACTORS IN SPATIAL CONTRAST SENSITIVITY 35 FIGURE 13 Contrast sensitivity as a function of the test field size in degrees of visual angle. The number of cycles in each stimulus is the product of the field size and the spatial frequency. Based on data reported by McCann, Savoy and Hall. SOURCE: McCann et al. (1978). FIGURE 14 Contrast sensitivity functions for long exposure time with gradual onset and offset (sustained presentation) and for short exposure time with rapid onset and offset (transient presentation). Note that low frequency sensitivity is enhanced and high frequency sensitivity is reduced with transient presentation. Based on data reported by McCann, Savoy, and Hall.

APPENDIX A: BASIC FACTORS IN SPATIAL CONTRAST SENSITIVITY 36 FIGURE 15 Contrast sensitivity as a function of both spatial and temporal frequency. Each individual curve represents the spatial frequency response at a fixed temporal frequency. SOURCE: Kelly, 1979. Reprinted with permission from D. H. Kelly. Copyright 1979 by the American Institute of Physics in the Journal of the Optical Society of America 69. FIGURE 16 Contrast sensitivity function for vertical and oblique orientations. Note the lower sensitivity at oblique orientation. SOURCE: Campbell et al., 1966. Reprinted with permission from F. W. Campbell. Copyright 1966 by The Physiological Society.

Next: APPENDIX B: DETECTION SENSITIVITY AND RESPONSE BIAS »
Emergent Techniques for Assessment of Visual Performance Get This Book
×
Buy Paperback | $40.00
MyNAP members save 10% online.
Login or Register to save!
Download Free PDF

Recent vision research has led to the emergence of new techniques that offer exciting potential for a more complete assessment of vision in clinical, industrial, and military settings. Emergent Techniques for Assessment of Visual Performance examines four areas of vision testing that offer potential for improved assessment of visual capability including: contrast sensitivity function, dark-focus of accommodation, dynamic visual acuity and dynamic depth tracking, and ambient and focal vision. In contrast to studies of accepted practices, this report focuses on emerging techniques that could help determine whether people have the vision necessary to do their jobs. In addition to examining some of these emerging techniques, the report identifies their usefulness in predicting performance on other visual and visual-motor tasks, and makes recommendations for future research.

Emergent Techniques for Assessment of Visual Performance provides summary recommendations for research that will have significant value and policy implications for the next 5 to 10 years. The content and conclusions of this report can serve as a useful resource for those responsible for screening industrial and military visual function.

  1. ×

    Welcome to OpenBook!

    You're looking at OpenBook, NAP.edu's online reading room since 1999. Based on feedback from you, our users, we've made some improvements that make it easier than ever to read thousands of publications on our website.

    Do you want to take a quick tour of the OpenBook's features?

    No Thanks Take a Tour »
  2. ×

    Show this book's table of contents, where you can jump to any chapter by name.

    « Back Next »
  3. ×

    ...or use these buttons to go back to the previous chapter or skip to the next one.

    « Back Next »
  4. ×

    Jump up to the previous page or down to the next one. Also, you can type in a page number and press Enter to go directly to that page in the book.

    « Back Next »
  5. ×

    To search the entire text of this book, type in your search term here and press Enter.

    « Back Next »
  6. ×

    Share a link to this book page on your preferred social network or via email.

    « Back Next »
  7. ×

    View our suggested citation for this chapter.

    « Back Next »
  8. ×

    Ready to take your reading offline? Click here to buy this book in print or download it as a free PDF, if available.

    « Back Next »
Stay Connected!