![]() Catherine E. Snow, M. Susan Burns,
Committee on the Prevention of Reading
Commission on Behavioral and
National Research Council NATIONAL ACADEMY PRESS
NATIONAL ACADEMY PRESS 2101 Constitution Avenue, N.W. Washington, D.C. 20418
NOTICE:
The study was supported by Grant No. H023S50001 between the National Academy of Sciences and the U.S. Department of Education. Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations expressed in this publication are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the views of the organizations or agencies that provided support for this project.
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
Committee on the Prevention of Reading Difficulties in Young Children. Preventing reading difficulties in young children / Committee on the Prevention of Reading Difficulties in Young Children ; Catherine E. Snow, M. Susan Burns, and Peg Griffin, editors.
Includes bibliographical references and index.
1. Reading (Primary)United States. 2. Reading disabilityUnited States. 3. ReadingRemedial teachingUnited States. 4. Reading comprehensionUnited States. 5. Word recognition. I. Snow, Catherine E. II. Burns, M. Susan (Marie Susan) III. Griffin, Peg. IV. Title. LB1525.76 .C66 1998 372.4ddc21 98-9031
Additional copies of this report are available from National Academy Press, 2101 Constitution Avenue, N.W., Lockbox 285, Washington, D.C. 20055. Call (800) 624-6242 or (202) 334-3313 (in the Washington metropolitan area). This report is also available online at http://www.nap.edu
Printed in the United States of America Copyright 1998 by the National Academy of Sciences. All rights reserved.
Committee on the Prevention of Reading Difficulties in Young Children CATHERINE SNOW (Chair), Graduate School of Education, Harvard University MARILYN JAGER ADAMS, GTE Internet Working Group, and Visiting Scholar, Harvard University BARBARA T. BOWMAN, Erikson Institute, Chicago, Illinois BARBARA FOORMAN, Department of Pediatrics, University of Texas, and Houston Medical School DOROTHY FOWLER, Fairfax County Public Schools, Annandale, Virginia CLAUDE N. GOLDENBERG, Department of Teacher Education, California State University, Long Beach EDWARD J. KAME'ENUI, Department of Special Education, University of Oregon, Eugene WILLIAM LABOV, Department of Linguistics and Psychology, University of Pennsylvania RICHARD K. OLSON, Department of Psychology, University of Colorado, Boulder ANNEMARIE SULLIVAN PALINCSAR, School of Education, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor CHARLES A. PERFETTI, Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh HOLLIS S. SCARBOROUGH, Brooklyn College, City University of New York, and Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, Connecticut SALLY SHAYWITZ, Department of Pediatrics, Yale University KEITH STANOVICH, Ontario Institute for Studies in Education, University of Toronto DOROTHY STRICKLAND, Graduate School of Education, Rutgers University SAM STRINGFIELD, Center for the Social Organization of Schools, Johns Hopkins University ELIZABETH SULZBY, School of Education, University of
Michigan, Ann Arbor
M. SUSAN BURNS, Study Director PEG GRIFFIN, Research Associate SHARON VANDIVERE, Project Assistant
Preface "Few things in life are less efficient than a group of people trying to write a sentence" (Scott Adams, The Dilbert Principle, 1996). The decision that a group of people should write a report of this size clearly was not motivated by the goal of efficiency; it was motivated by the goals of comprehensiveness and accuracy and made feasible by the expectation of compromise and consensus. The field of reading is one that has long been marked by controversies and disagreements. Indeed, the term "reading wars" has been part of the debate over reading research for the past 25 years. The unpleasantness of the conflicts among reading researchers was moderated, if not eliminated, by the realization that all the participants are primarily interested in ensuring the well-being of young children and in promoting optimal literacy instruction. The study reported in this volume was undertaken with the assumption that empirical work in the field of reading had advanced sufficiently to allow substantial agreed-upon results and conclusions that could form a basis for breaching the differences among the warring parties. The process of doing the study revealed the correctness of the assumption that this has been an appropriate time to undertake a synthesis of the research on early reading development. The knowledge base is now large enough that the controversies that have dominated discussions of reading development and reading instruction have given way to a widely honored pax lectura, the conditions of which include a shared focus on the needs and rights of all children to learn to read. Under the treaties that have recently been entered into, furthermore, the focus of attention has shifted from the researchers' theories and data back to the teacher, alone in her classroom with a heterogeneous group of children, all awaiting their passports to literacy. From the perspective of the teacher, our task can be conceptualized as cutting through the detail of partially convergent, sometimes discrepant research findings to provide an integrated picture of how reading develops and how reading instruction should proceed. It may come as a surprise to the reader to find that consensus in achieving that integrated picture, among the members of this diverse committee, was not difficult to reach. All members agreed that reading should be defined as a process of getting meaning from print, using knowledge about the written alphabet and about the sound structure of oral language for purposes of achieving understanding. All thus also agreed that early reading instruction should include direct teaching of information about sound-symbol relationships to children who do not know about them and that it must also maintain a focus on the communicative purposes and personal value of reading. In this report, the committee makes recommendations for practice, as well as recommendations for further research that needs to be undertaken. Our discussions also explored how people need to start thinking about reading and reading instruction. This turned out to be harder to formulate, because it evokes the often frustrating and familiarly academic position that "this is an incredibly complicated phenomenon." Although we can see the readers' eyes rolling at the predictability of this claim, we nonetheless persist in the contention that much of the difficulty in seeking real reforms in reading instruction and intervention derives from simplistic beliefs about these issues, and so one step in improving matters involves making the complexities known. Not only the first-grade teacher, but also the parent, the pediatrician, the school administrator, the curriculum consultant, the textbook publisher, the state legislator, and the secretary of education need to understand both what is truly hard about learning to read and how wide-ranging and varied the experiences are that support and facilitate reading acquisition. All these people need to understand as well that many factors that correlate with reading fail to explain it, that many experiences contribute to reading development without being prerequisite to it, and that there are many prerequisites, so no single one can be considered sufficient. The focus of this report is prevention. We thus try to sketch a picture of the conditions under which reading is most likely to develop easilyconditions that include stimulating preschool environments, excellent reading instruction, and the absence of any of a wide array of risk factors. Our focus on trying to provide optimal conditions does not mean that we think that children experiencing less than optimal conditions are in any sense doomed to failure in reading; many children from poor and uneducated families learn to read well, even without excellent preschool classroom experience or superb early reading instruction. Nonetheless, with an eye to reducing risk and preventing failure, we focus on mechanisms for providing the best possible situation for every child. We submit this report with high hopes that it may indeed
mark the end of the reading wars and that it will contribute to the
successful reading development of many children. It is the collective
product of the entire committee, and it could not have been
produced without the selfless contributions of time, thought, and hard work
of all members, or without their willingness to confront with
integrity and resolve with grace their many productive disagreements
with one another.
Acknowledgments Many people contributed in many different ways to the completion of this report, and we are most grateful for their efforts. First, the committee and staff would like to acknowledge Ellen Schiller (U.S. Department of Education), Naomi Karp (U.S. Department of Education), and Reid Lyon (National Institutes of Health) for assistance given during the project. Judith Heumann, Tom Hehir, and Louis Danielson of the Office of Special Education (U.S. Department of Education) and Duane Alexander of the National Institutes of Health provided support and encouragement. Our thanks to Rebecca Fitch (U.S. Department of Education) and Fritz Mosher (Carnegie Corporation) for their help in developing plans for liaison activities. During the information-gathering phase of our work, a number of people made presentations to the committee on programs that focused on the prevention of reading difficulties: Steve Barnett (moderator); Elizabeth Segal (Beginning with Books); Marcia Invernizzi (Book Buddies); Andrew Hayes (Comprehensive Family Literacy Program); John Guthrie (Concept-Oriented Reading Instruction); Bob Stark (Early ID: Reading Early Identification and Intervention); Barbara Taylor (Early Intervention in Reading); Jerry Zimmerman and Carolyn Brown (Breakthrough to Literacy); Sabra Gelfond (HAILO); Annette Dove (Home Instruction Program for Preschool YoungstersHIPPY); Pia Rebello (Home Instruction Program for Preschool YoungstersHIPPY); Darcy Vogel (Intergenerational Tutoring Program); Ethna Reid (Keyboarding, Reading, Spelling); Bob Lemire and Kathy Hook (Phonics-Based Reading); George Farkas (Reading One-One); M. Trika Smith-Burke (Reading Recovery); James Wendorf, Linda Gambrell, and Suzanne Kealey (RIF's Running Start Program); John Nunnary (Success For All); Marilyn Howard (Auditory Discrimination in Depth). In addition, materials and advice were provided by various programs and professional and advocacy groups. The program materials include: 4 H Family; Books Aloud; Books and Beyond; Children's Television Workshop/Sesame Street; Class-Wide Peer Tutoring; Cornell Cooperative Extension; Dyslexia Training Program; Expeditionary Learning Outward Bound; First Book; Four Blocks; Four Remedial Reading Programs; Friend/Non-Friend; Getting Books in Children's Hands; Ladders to Literacy: Kindergarten; Ladders to Literacy: Preschool; Listening Comprehension; Little Planet Publishing; National Reading Research Center-Georgia; National Speech Language Therapy Center; New Chance; Open Court; Project Read Program; Project Seed; Reach Out and Read; Readnet: Pathways to Literacy Readnet Foundation; Stony Brook Reading and Language; Sound Partners; Waterford Early Reading Program; Wiggle Works; and Youth Opportunities Unlimited. The professional associations and other groups include the American Association of Colleges for Teacher Education; American Humane Association; American Library Association; American Psychological Association, Division 37; Child, Youth and Family Services; American Public Welfare Association; Big Brothers/Big Sisters of America; Child Welfare League of America; Families, 4-H and Nutrition; Home and School Institute; Initiatives for Children; Institute for Educational Leadership; International Reading Association; Learning Disabilities Association; National 4-H Council; National Association for Bilingual Education; National Association of Elementary School Principals; National Association of School Psychologists; National Association of State Boards of Education; National Center for Education in Maternal and Child Health; National Center for Family Literacy; National Center for Immigrant Students; National Education Association; National School Boards Association; Save the Children International; and School-Age Child Care Project. The panel was assisted in its deliberations by a number of people who wrote background papers: Steven Barnett, "The Effects of Preschool Programs on Reading Achievement"; Lynn Fuchs, "Monitoring Student Progress Toward the Development of Reading Competence: Classroom-Based Assessment Methods"; Stanley Herr, "Special Education Law and Young Children with Reading Disabilities or at Risk of Such Disabilities"; Laura Klenk, "Review of Selected Remediation and Early Intervention Programs"; James McClelland, "The Basis and Remediation of Language Impairments in Children"; Kevin S. McGrew, "The Measurement of Reading Achievement by Different Individually Administered Standardized Reading Tests: Apples and Apples, or Apples and Oranges?"; Robert Needlman, "Pediatric Interventions to Prevent Reading Problems in Young Children"; Carol Padden, "Reading and Deafness"; Bennett A. Shaywitz, "The Neurobiology of Reading and Reading Disability"; Margaret J. Snowling, "A Review of the Literature on Reading, Informed by PDP Models, with Special Regard to Children Between Birth and Age 8, Who May Be at Risk of Not Learning to Read." Numerous researchers also shared their work with the committee, including Catherine Dorsey-Gaines (Kean College of New Jersey); Vivian L. Gadsden (University of Pennsylvania); Russell Gersten (University of Oregon); Robert Rueda (University of Southern California); Rune J. Simeonsson (University of North Carolina); Frank R. Vellutino (State University of New York, Albany); and special education project directors present at our information-gathering meeting in July 1996. This report has been reviewed by individuals chosen for their diverse perspectives and technical expertise, in accordance with procedures approved by the National Research Council's Report Review Committee. The purpose of this independent review is to provide candid and critical comments that will assist the authors and the National Research Council in making the published report as sound as possible and to ensure that the report meets institutional standards for objectivity, evidence, and responsiveness to the study charge. The content of the review comments and draft manuscript remain confidential to protect the integrity of the deliberative process. We wish to thank the following individuals for their participation in the review of this report: Benita A. Blachman, School of Education, Syracuse University; Peter Bryant, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford; Courtney Cazden, Graduate School of Education, Harvard University; Britton Chance, Departments of Biochemistry and Biophysics (emeritus), University of Pennsylvania; Ruth Fielding-Barnsley, Psychology Department, University of New England, New South Wales, Australia; John Guthrie, College of Education, University of Maryland, College Park; Eileen Kowler, Department of Psychology, Rutgers University; Frank Manis, Department of Psychology, University of Southern California; Luis Moll, Division of Language, Reading, and Culture, University of Arizona; P. David Pearson, College of Education, Michigan State University; W. Charles Read, School of Education, University of Wisconsin, Madison; Patrick Suppes, Center for the Study of Language and Information (emeritus), Stanford University; Richard Wagner, Department of Psychology, Florida State University; and Grover J. Whitehurst, Department of Psychology, State University of New York, Stony Brook. While the individuals listed above have provided many constructive comments and suggestions, responsibility for the final content of this report rests solely with the authoring committee and the National Research Council. Throughout the research, conceptualization, and writing
phase of this work, our coeditor, Peg Griffin, was an invaluable
colleaguea strong-minded collaborator, a tireless writer, and a reliably
good-natured colleague. Alexandra Wigdor, director of the Division
on Education, Labor, and Human Performance, and Janet Hansen,
also of the division, provided guidance and support throughout
the project. This final product has benefited enormously from the
editorial attention of Christine McShane. Marie Suizzo, Marilyn
Dabady, Roger Butts, and Sharon Vandivere ably assisted the committee
at different stages. The committee extends its sincere thanks and
appreciation to all those who assisted us in our work.
Contents PART I INTRODUCTION TO READING 1 Introduction
2 The Process of Learning to Read
PART II WHO ARE WE TALKING ABOUT? 3 Who Has Reading Difficulties?
4 Predictors of Success and Failure in Reading
PART III PREVENTION AND INTERVENTION 5 Preventing Reading Difficulties Before Kindergarten
6 Instructional Strategies for Kindergarten and the Primary Grades 7 Organizational Strategies for Kindergarten and the Primary Grades
8 Helping Children with Reading Difficulties in Grades 1 to 3
PART IV KNOWLEDGE INTO ACTION 9 The Agents of Change
10 Recommendations for Practice and Research
References
Biographical Sketches
Index
Reading is essential to success in our society. The ability to read is highly valued and important for social and economic advancement. Of course, most children learn to read fairly well. In this report, we are most concerned with the large numbers of children in America whose educational careers are imperiled because they do not read well enough to ensure understanding and to meet the demands of an increasingly competitive economy. Current difficulties in reading largely originate from rising demands for literacy, not from declining absolute levels of literacy. In a technological society, the demands for higher literacy are ever increasing, creating more grievous consequences for those who fall short. The importance of this problem led the U.S. Department of
Education and the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services
to ask the National Academy of Sciences to establish a committee
to examine the prevention of reading difficulties. Our committee
was charged with conducting a study of the effectiveness of
interventions for young children who are at risk of having problems learning
to read. The goals of the project were three: (1) to comprehend a
rich but diverse research base; (2) to translate the research findings
into advice and guidance for parents, educators, publishers, and
others involved in the care and instruction of the young; and (3) to
convey this advice to the targeted audiences through a variety of
publications, conferences, and other outreach activities.
THE COMMITTEE'S APPROACH The committee reviewed research on normal reading development and instruction; on risk factors useful in identifying groups and individuals at risk of reading failure; and on prevention, intervention, and instructional approaches to ensuring optimal reading outcomes. We found many informative literatures to draw on and have aimed in this report to weave together the insights of many research traditions into clear guidelines for helping children become successful readers. In doing so, we also considered the current state of affairs in education for teachers and others working with young children; policies of federal, state, and local governments impinging on young children's education; the pressures on publishers of curriculum materials, texts, and tests; programs addressed to parents and to community action; and media activities. Our main emphasis has been on the development of reading and on factors that relate to reading outcomes. We conceptualized our task as cutting through the detail of mostly convergent, but sometimes discrepant, research findings to provide an integrated picture of how reading develops and how its development can be promoted. Our recommendations extend to all children. Granted, we have focused our lens on children at risk for learning to read. But much of the instructional research we have reviewed encompasses, for a variety of reasons, populations of students with varying degrees of risk. Good instruction seems to transcend characterizations of children's vulnerability for failure; the same good early literacy environment and patterns of effective instruction are required for children who might fail for different reasons. Does this mean that the identical mix of instructional
materials and strategies will work for each and every child? Of course not.
If we have learned anything from this effort, it is that effective
teachers are able to craft a special mix of instructional ingredients for
every child they work with. But it does mean that there is a common
menu of materials, strategies, and environments from which effective
teachers make choices. This in turn means that, as a society, our
most important challenge is to make sure that our teachers have access
to those tools and the knowledge required to use them well. In
other words, there is little evidence that children experiencing
difficulties learning to read, even those with identifiable learning
disabilities, need radically different sorts of supports than children at low
risk, although they may need much more intensive support.
Childhood environments that support early literacy development and
excellent instruction are important for all children. Excellent instruction
is the best intervention for children who demonstrate problems
learning to read.
CONCEPTUALIZING READING AND Effective reading instruction is built on a foundation that recognizes that reading ability is determined by multiple factors: many factors that correlate with reading fail to explain it; many experiences contribute to reading development without being prerequisite to it; and although there are many prerequisites, none by itself is considered sufficient. Adequate initial reading instruction requires that children:
Adequate progress in learning to read English (or any alphabetic language) beyond the initial level depends on:
Reading skill is acquired in a relatively predictable way by children who have normal or above-average language skills; have had experiences in early childhood that fostered motivation and provided exposure to literacy in use; get information about the nature of print through opportunities to learn letters and to recognize the internal structure of spoken words, as well as explanations about the contrasting nature of spoken and written language; and attend schools that provide effective reading instruction and opportunities to practice reading. Disruption of any of these developments increases the possibility that reading will be delayed or impeded. The association of poor reading outcomes with poverty and minority status no doubt reflects the accumulated effects of several of these risk factors, including lack of access to literacy-stimulating preschool experiences and to excellent, coherent reading instruction. In addition, a number of children without any obvious risk factors also develop reading difficulties. These children may require intensive efforts at intervention and extra help in reading and accommodations for their disability throughout their lives. There are three potential stumbling blocks that are known to throw children off course on the journey to skilled reading. The first obstacle, which arises at the outset of reading acquisition, is difficulty understanding and using the alphabetic principlethe idea that written spellings systematically represent spoken words. It is hard to comprehend connected text if word recognition is inaccurate or laborious. The second obstacle is a failure to transfer the comprehension skills of spoken language to reading and to acquire new strategies that may be specifically needed for reading. The third obstacle to reading will magnify the first two: the absence or loss of an initial motivation to read or failure to develop a mature appreciation of the rewards of reading. As in every domain of learning, motivation is crucial. Although most children begin school with positive attitudes and expectations for success, by the end of the primary grades and increasingly thereafter, some children become disaffected. The majority of reading problems faced by today's adolescents and adults are the result of problems that might have been avoided or resolved in their early childhood years. It is imperative that steps be taken to ensure that children overcome these obstacles during the primary grades. Reducing the number of children who enter school with
inadequate literacy-related knowledge and skill is an important
primary step toward preventing reading difficulties. Although not a
panacea, this would serve to reduce considerably the magnitude of the
problem currently facing schools. Children who are particularly likely
to have difficulty with learning to read in the primary grades are
those who begin school with less prior knowledge and skill in
relevant domains, most notably general verbal abilities, the ability to
attend to the sounds of language as distinct from its meaning,
familiarity with the basic purposes and mechanisms of reading, and letter
knowledge. Children from poor neighborhoods, children with
limited proficiency in English, children with hearing impairments,
children with preschool language impairments, and children whose
parents had difficulty learning to read are particularly at risk of arriving
at school with weaknesses in these areas and hence of falling
behind from the outset.
RECOMMENDATIONS The critical importance of providing excellent reading
instruction to all children is at the heart of the committee's
recommendations. Accordingly, our central recommendation characterizes
the nature of good primary reading instruction. We also recognize
that excellent instruction is most effective when children arrive in
first grade motivated for literacy and with the necessary linguistic,
cognitive, and early literacy skills. We therefore recommend attention
to ensuring high-quality preschool and kindergarten environments
as well. We acknowledge that excellent instruction in the
primary grades and optimal environments in preschool and kindergarten
require teachers who are well prepared, highly knowledgeable,
and receiving ongoing support. Excellent instruction may be
possible only if schools are organized in optimal ways; if facilities,
curriculum materials, and support services function adequately; and if
children's home languages are taken into account in designing instruction.
We therefore make recommendations addressing these issues. (The
complete text of all the committee's recommendations appears in
Chapter 10.)
Literacy Instruction in First Through Third Grades Given the centrality of excellent instruction to the prevention of reading difficulties, the committee strongly recommends attention in every primary-grade classroom to the full array of early reading accomplishments: the alphabetic principle, reading sight words, reading words by mapping speech sounds to parts of words, achieving fluency, and comprehension. Getting started in alphabetic reading depends critically on mapping the letters and spellings of words onto the speech units that they represent; failure to master word recognition can impede text comprehension. Explicit instruction that directs children's attention to the sound structure of oral language and to the connections between speech sounds and spellings assists children who have not grasped the alphabetic principle or who do not apply it productively when they encounter unfamiliar printed words. Comprehension difficulties can be prevented by actively building comprehension skills as well as linguistic and conceptual knowledge, beginning in the earliest grades. Comprehension can be enhanced through instruction focused on concept and vocabulary growth and background knowledge, instruction about the syntax and rhetorical structures of written language, and direct instruction about comprehension strategies such as summarizing, predicting, and monitoring. Comprehension also takes practice, which is gained by reading independently, by reading in pairs or groups, and by being read aloud to. We recommend that first through third grade curricula include the following components:
Promoting Literacy Development in Preschool and Kindergarten It is clear from the research that the process of learning to read is a lengthy one that begins very early in life. Given the importance identified in the research literature of starting school motivated to read and with the prerequisite language and early literacy skills, the committee recommends that all children, especially those at risk for reading difficulties, should have access to early childhood environments that promote language and literacy growth and that address a variety of skills that have been identified as predictors of later reading achievement. Preschools and other group care settings for young children often provide relatively impoverished language and literacy environments, in particular those available to families with limited economic resources. As ever more young children are entering group care settings pursuant to expectations that their mothers will join the work force, it becomes critical that the preschool opportunities available to lower-income families be designed in ways that support language and literacy development. Preschool programs, even those designed specifically as interventions for children at risk of reading difficulties, should be designed to provide optimal support for cognitive, language, and social development, within this broad focus. However, ample attention should be paid to skills that are known to predict future reading achievement, especially those for which a causal role has been demonstrated. Similarly, and for the same reasons, kindergarten instruction should be designed to stimulate verbal interaction; to enrich children's vocabularies; to encourage talk about books; to provide practice with the sound structure of words; to develop knowledge about print, including the production and recognition of letters; and to generate familiarity with the basic purposes and mechanisms of reading. Children who will probably need additional support for
early language and literacy development should receive it as early as
possible. Pediatricians, social workers, speech-language therapists,
and other preschool practitioners should receive research-based
guidelines to assist them to be alert for signs that children are
having difficulties acquiring early language and literacy skills. Parents,
relatives, neighbors, and friends can also play a role in identifying
children who need assistance. Through adult education programs,
public service media, instructional videos provided by pediatricians,
and other means, parents can be informed about what skills and
knowledge children should be acquiring at young ages, and about what
to do and where to turn if there is concern that a child's
development may be lagging behind in some respects.
Education and Professional Development for All Involved in Literacy Instruction The critical importance of the teacher in the prevention of reading difficulties must be recognized, and efforts should be made to provide all teachers with adequate knowledge about reading and the knowledge and skill to teach reading or its developmental precursors. It is imperative that teachers at all grade levels understand the course of literacy development and the role of instruction in optimizing literacy development. Preschool teachers represent an important, and largely under-utilized, resource in promoting literacy by supporting rich language and emergent literacy skills. Early childhood educators should not try to replicate the formal reading instruction provided in schools. The preschool and primary school teacher's knowledge and experience, as well as the support provided to the teacher, are central to achieving the goal of primary prevention of reading difficulties. Each of these may vary according to where the teacher is in his or her professional development. A critical component in the preparation of pre-service teachers is supervised, relevant, clinical experience providing ongoing guidance and feedback, so they develop the ability to integrate and apply their knowledge in practice. Teachers need to be knowledgeable about the research foundations of reading. Collaborative support by the teacher preparation institution and the field placement is essential. A critical component for novice teachers is the support of mentors who have demonstrated records of success in teaching reading. Professional development should not be conceived as
something that ends with graduation from a teacher preparation program,
nor as something that happens primarily in graduate classrooms or
even during in-service activities. Rather, ongoing support from
colleagues and specialists, as well as regular opportunities for
self-examination and reflection, are critical components of the career-long
development of excellent teachers.
Teaching Reading to Speakers of Other Languages Schools have the responsibility to accommodate the linguistic needs of students with limited proficiency in English. Precisely how to do this is difficult to prescribe, because students' abilities and needs vary greatly, as do the capacities of different communities to support their literacy development. The committee recommends the following guidelines for decision making:
Ensuring Adequate Resources to Meet Children's Needs To be effective, schools with large numbers of children at risk for reading difficulties need rich resourcesmanageable class sizes and student-teacher ratios, high-quality instructional materials in sufficient quantity, good school libraries, and pleasant physical environments. Achieving this may require extra resources for schools that serve a disproportionate number of high-risk children. Even in schools in which a large percentage of the students
are not achieving at a satisfactory level, a well-designed classroom
reading program, delivered by an experienced and competent
teacher, may be successful in bringing most students to grade level or
above during the primary grades. However, achieving and sustaining
radical gains is often difficult when improvements are introduced on
a classroom-by-classroom basis. In a situation of school-wide
poor performance, school restructuring should be considered as a
vehicle for preventing reading difficulties. Ongoing professional
development for teachers is typically a component of successful school
restructuring efforts.
Addressing the Needs of Children with Even with excellent instruction in the early grades, some children fail to make satisfactory progress in reading. Such children will require supplementary services, ideally from a reading specialist who provides individual or small-group intensive instruction that is coordinated with high-quality instruction from the classroom teacher. Children who are having difficulty learning to read do not, as a rule, require qualitatively different instruction from children who are "getting it." Instead, they more often need application of the same principles by someone who can apply them expertly to individual children who are having difficulty for one reason or another. Schools that lack or have abandoned reading specialist positions need to reexamine their needs for such specialists to ensure that well-trained staff are available for intervention with children and for ongoing support to classroom teachers. Reading specialists and other specialist roles need to be defined so that two-way communication is required between specialists and classroom teachers about the needs of all children at risk of or experiencing reading difficulties. Coordination is needed at the instructional level so that intervention from specialists coordinates with and supports classroom instruction. Schools that have reading specialists as well as special educators need to coordinate the roles of these specialists. Schools need to ensure that all the specialists engaged in child study or individualized educational program (IEP) meetings for special education placement, early childhood intervention, out-of-classroom interventions, or in-classroom support are well informed about research in reading development and the prevention of reading difficulties. Although volunteer tutors can provide valuable practice
and motivational support for children learning to read, they should
not be expected either to provide primary reading instruction or to
instruct children with serious reading problems.
CONCLUSION Most reading difficulties can be prevented. There is much
work to be done, however, that requires the aggressive deployment of
the information currently available, which is distilled in this report.
In addition, many questions remain unanswered concerning
reading development, some of which we address in our recommendations
for research. While science continues to discover more about how
children learn to read and how teachers and others can help them,
the knowledge currently available can equip our society to
promote higher levels of literacy for large numbers of American
schoolchildren. The committee's hope is that the recommendations
contained in this report will provide direction for the first important steps. |